atical
     Sponsored by the Georgia Center
       for the Study of Learning and
          Teaching  Mathematics
                  and the
   Department of Mathematics Education
           University of Georgia
              Athens, Georgia

          Larry L, Hatfield, Editor
     David A. Bradbard, Technical Editor
ERIC/SMEAC CENTER FOR SCIENCE, MATHEMATICS,
AND ENVIRONMENTAL EDUCATION.
    an information center to organize and disseminate
information and materials on science, mathematics, and
environmental education  to teachers, administrators,
supervisors, researchers, and the public. A joint project of
the College of Education, The Ohio State University and
the Educational Resources Information Center of NIE.

-------
MATHEMATICAL

PROBLEM

SOLVING
                               Papers from a Research Workshop
Sponsored by the Georgia Center
for the Study of Learning and
Teaching Mathematics
     and the
Department of Mathematics Education
University of Georgia
Athens, Georgia

Larry L. Hatfield,  Editor
David A. Bradbard,  Technical Editor
         ERIC  Clearinghouse  for Science, Mathematics,
                   and Environmental Education
                      College of Education
                    The Ohio  State University
                 1200 Chambers Road, Third Floor
                      Columbus, Ohio 43212
                          January  1978

-------
These papers were prepared as part of the activities of the Georgia
Center for the Study of Learning and Teaching Mathematics,  under
Grant No. PES 7418491, National Science Foundation.  The opinions
expressed herein do not necessarily reflect the position or policy
of the National Science Foundation.

This publication was prepared pursuant to a contract with the National
Institute of Education, U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare.
Contractors undertaking such projects under Government sponsorship
are encouraged to express freely their judgment in professional
and technical matters.  Points of view or opinions do not,  therefore,
necessarily represent official National Institute of Education position
or policy.
                                  ii

-------
                MATHEMATICS EDUCATION REPORTS


     The Mathematics Education Reports series makes available recent

analyses and syntheses of research and development efforts in mathe-

matics education.  We are pleased to make available as part of this

series the papers from the Workshop on Mathematical Problem Solving

sponsored by the Georgia Center for the Study of Learning and Teach-

ing Mathematics.

     Other Mathematics Education Reports make available information

concerning mathematics education documents analyzed at the ERIC

Information Analysis Center for Science, Mathematics, and Environ-

mental Education.  These reports fall into three broad categories-

Research reviews summarize and analyze recent research in specific

areas of mathematics education.  Resource guides identify and analyze

materials and references for use by mathematics teachers at all

levels.  Special bibliographies announce the availability of docu-

ments and review the literature in selected  interest areas of

mathematics education.

     Priorities  for  the development of  future Mathematics Education

Reports are established by the advisory board of the Center, in

cooperation with the National Council of Teachers  of Mathematics,

the Special Interest Group for Research in Mathematics Education,

and other professional groups in mathematics education.  Individual

comments on past Reports and suggestions for future Reports are

always welcomed by our Clearinghouse.
                                           Jon L. Higgins
                                           Associate Director  for
                                            Mathematics Education

                               iii

-------
                                Contents
Acknowledgements and Overview
   Leslie P. Steffe,  Thomas J. Cooney,  and Larry L.  Hatfield  	   vii

Research on Mathematical Problem Solving:   An Overview
   Larry L. Hatfield  	      1
Variables and Methodologies in Research on Problem Solving
   Jeremy Kilpatrick 	      ?

Hnuristical Emphases in the Instruction of Mathematical Problem
Solving:  Rationales and Research
   Larry L. Hatfield 	     21

The Teaching Experiment and Soviet Studies of Problem Solving
   Mary Grace Kantowski 	     ^3
Mathematical Problem Solving in the Elementary School:  Some
Educational and Psychological Considerations
   Frank K. Lester, Jr	    53

-------
                      Acknowledgements and Overview
     The Georgia Center for the Study of Learning and Teaching Mathematics
(GCSLTM) was started July 1, 1975, through a founding grant from the
National Science Foundation.  Various activities preceded the founding
of the GCSLTM.  The most significant was a conference held at Columbia
University in October of 1970 on Piagetian Cognitive-Development and
Mathematical Education.  This conference was directed by the late Myron
F. Rosskopf and jointly sponsored by the National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics and the Department of Mathematical Education, Teachers
College, Columbia University with a grant from the National Science
Foundation.  Following the October 1970 Conference, Professor Rosskopf
spent the winter and spring quarters of 1971 as a visiting professor of
Mathematics Education at the University of Georgia.  During these two
quarters, the editorial work was accomplished on the proceedings of the
October conference and a Letter of Intent was filed in February of 1971
with the National Science Foundation to create a Center for Mathematical
Education Research and Innovation.  Professor Rosskopf's illness and
untimely death made it impossible for him to develop the ideas contained
in that Letter.

     After much discussion among faculty in the Department of Mathematics
Education at the University of Georgia, it was clear that a center devoted
to•the study of mathematics education ought to attack a broader range of
problems than was stated in the Letter of Intent.  As a result of these
discussions, three areas of study were identified as being of primary
interest in the initial year of the Georgia Center for the Study of
Learning and Teaching Mathematics—Teaching Strategies, Concept Develop-
ment, and Problem Solving.  Thomas J. Cooney assumed directorship of the
Teaching Strategies Project, Leslie P. Steffe the Concept Development
Project, and Larry L. Hatfield the Problem Solving Project.

     The GCSLTM is intended to be a long-term operation with the broad
goal of improving mathematics education in elementary and secondary schools.
To be effective, it was felt that the Center would have to include
mathematics educators with interests commensurate with those of the
project areas.  Alternative organizational patterns were available—
resident scholars, institutional consortia, or individual consortia.
The latter organizational pattern was chosen because it was felt maximum
participation would be then possible.  In order to operational!ze a
concept of a consortia of individuals, five research workshops were held
during the spring of 1975 at the University of Georgia.  These workshops
were (ordered by dates held) Teaching Strategies, Number and Measurement
Concepts, Space and Geometry Concepts, Models for Learning Mathematics,
                                vii

-------
and Problem Solving.  Papers were commissioned for each workshop.   It
was necessary to commission papers for two reasons.   First,  current
analyses and syntheses of the knowledge in the particular areas chosen
for investigation were needed.  Second, catalysts  for further  research
and development activities were needed—major problems had to  be
identified in the project areas on which work was  needed.

     Twelve working groups have emerged from these workshops,  three in
Teaching Strategies, five in Concept Development,  and four in  Problem
Solving.  The three working groups in Teaching Strategies are:  Differential
Effects of Varying Teaching Strategies, John Dossey, Coordinator;
Development of Protocol Materials to Depict Moves  and Strategies,  Kenneth
Retzer, Coordinator; and Investigation of Certain  Teacher Behavior That
May Be Associated with Effective Teaching, Thomas  J. Cooney, Coordinator.
The five working groups in Concept Development are:   Measurement Concepts,
Thomas Romberg, Coordinator; Rational Number Concepts, Thomas  Kieren,
Coordinator; Cardinal and Ordinal Number Concepts, Leslie P. Steffe,
Coordinator; Space and Geometry Concepts, Richard  Lesh, Coordinator; and
Models for Learning Mathematics, William Geeslin,  Coordinator.  The
four working groups in Problem Solving are:  Instruction in the Use of
Key Organizers (Single Heuristics), Frank Lester,  Coordinator; Instruction
Organized to use Heuristics in Combinations, Phillip Smith}  Coordinator;
Instruction in Problem Solving Strategies, Douglas Grouws, Coordinator;
and Task Variables for Problem Solving Research, Gerald Kulm,  Coordinator.
The twelve working groups are working as units somewhat independently
of one another.  As research and development emerges from working groups,
it is envisioned that some working groups will merge naturally.

    " The publication program of the Center is of central importance to
Center activities.  Research and development monographs and school mono-
graphs will be issued, when appropriate, by each working group.  The
school monographs will be written in nontechnical language and are to be
aimed at teacher educators and school personnel.  Reports of single
studies may be also published as technical reports.

     All of the above plans and aspirations would not be possible if it
were not for the existence of professional mathematics educators with
the expertise in and commitment  to research and development in mathematics
education.  The professional commitment of mathematics educators to the
betterment of mathematics education in the schools has been vastly under-
estimated.  In fact, the basic premise on which the GCSLTM is predicated
is that there are a significant number of professional mathematics
educators with a great deal of individual commitment to creative scholar-
ship.  There is no attempt on the part of the Center to buy this scholar-
ship—only to stimulate it and provide a setting in which it can flourish.
                                viii

-------
     The Center administration wishes to thank the individuals who wrote
the excellent papers for the workshops, the participants who made the work-
shops possible, and the National Science Foundation for supporting
financially the first year of Center operation.  Various individuals have
provided valuable assistance in preparing the papers given at the workshops
for publication.  Mr. David Bradbard provided technical editorship; Mrs.
Julie Wetherbee, Mrs. Elizabeth Platt, Mrs. Kay Abney, and Mrs. Cheryl
Hirstein, proved to be able typists; and Mr. Robert Petty drafted the
figures.  Mrs. Julie Wetherbee also provided expertise in the daily
operation of the Center during its first year.  One can only feel grateful
for the existence of such capable and hardworking people.
Thomas J.  Cooney           Leslie P. Steffe           Larry L. Hatfield
Director                   Director                   Director
Teaching Strategies        Concept Development        Problem Solving
                                and
                           Director, GCSLTM
                                Ix

-------
              Research on Mathematical Problem Solving:

                            An Overview

                         Larry L.  Hatfield

                       University of Georgia
     As human endeavors go, it is a complex task to help someone else to
become a better problem solver.  Yet teachers, parents,  and even children
routinely engage in this task.  And their efforts yield  some degree of
success with most learners—witness the increasing complexity of society
and the solving skills necessary to cope with and advance civilization.
But it is still largely a mystery why certain efforts with certain learners
seem to produce either lesser or greater results.  Attempts to describe
why or how a person solves a mathematical problem have resulted in rather
shallow, primitive and incomplete pictures.  Explanations of how mathe-
matical problem-solving competence builds across a person's experiences
are similarly thin.  Predictive theories of human problem solving are
non-existent.

     Most mathematics educators consider any learning goals relative to
problem solving to be of major importance.  The contributions which the
researcher might make to these goals require careful deliberation and
planning.  While this may have been also necessary in the past, the
research enterprise in mathematical education has not always built upon
a thoughtful analysis of the researchable problems and scholarly methods
of solution for studying the daily functioning of classroom instruction
and learning of mathematics.

     It appears that tremors of change may be assuming quake proportions
within certain sectors of educational research.  Reconstructionists, such
as Cronbach (1966,1975), Shulman (1970), Snow (1974), and Magoon (1977),
are calling for educational researchers to adopt philosophies and metho-
dologies that require a break  from contrived, laboratory-oriented settings.
They are advocating that the dominant context of formal education—the
classroom with groups of students studying standard schooling subjects—
must again become the experimental environment.  Current psychological
theories of learning are incapable of explaining or directing activities
in such classrooms.  Yet to the leading American psychologists at the
turn of this century the study of the educational process in classrooms
was the vital focus of their discipline.  The subsequent rejection of all
unobservable mental processes, which characterized the transformation of
psychology during the antimentalistic revolution, led to the psycholgists1
retreat to ivory-tower laboratories and to non-human subjects.

-------
     To a large extent the infant field of mathematics education research
continues to emulate the tactics and academic standards imposed by this
Fisherian tradition.  Prototypic studies are usually short, narrowly
circumscribed, focussed on behavioral outcomes, and quickly analyzed.
Though perhaps conducted in a classroom, the research methodology usually
ignores most of the complexity of that environment by intentionally ignor-
ing the constructive processes or the situational (environmental) variables.
Shulman (1970) discussed several aspects in the study of educational envi-
ronments.  He observed that if research is conducted in a setting with
characteristics  similar to school situations, then one may be able to
make reasonable extrapolations to the classroom milieu.  He urged a renewed
concern for external validity so that "the experimental conditions can
serve as a sample from which to make inferences to a population of external
conditions of interest" (1970, p.377).  In order to study the characteris-
tics and effects of educational environments, he recommended a "distinctive
features" analysis:

     To deal with the discontinuity between the settings of research
     and of educational application, a common language or set of
     terms for characterizing both experimental educational settings
     and curricula is needed.  Researchers must seriously strive to
     develop a means of analyzing the characteristics of both exper-
     imental and school settings into a complex of distinctive features
     so the task validity of any particular experiment can be estimated
     in terms of the particular criterion setting to which inferences
     are being made...

          I envisage ultimately a situation in which use of such a
     distinctive features approach would allow one to characterize
     the instructional settings to which a particular body of exper-
     imental research would most effectively be applicable.  Conversely,
     one could begin with a curriculum of interest and use such an
     approach to identify critical experiments that might be conducted
     to examine particular features of the complex curricular Gestalt.
     (Shulman, 1970, p. 379)

     Cronbach (1966),discussing the logic of experiments on "discovery
learning," observed that a particular educational tactic is part of a total
instructional system.  Placement in a context always in combination with
other tactics prohibits conclusions to be drawn about the tactic considered
simply fay itself.  Educational researchers are called upon to study an
educational tactic in its proper context.  The approaches used by Soviet
psychology through the conduct of "teaching experiments" offers an
important framework for such studies.

     In addition to returning the educational research spotlight onto
the classroom, Magoon (1977) advocated refocusing our research on another
overlooked aspect.  Based upon acceptance of cognitive views in psychology

-------
and sociology, he speculated that a construetivist perspective will
likely gain credibility among educational researchers.  Constructivism
assumes that knowledge is a phenomenon built-up within and by the human
subject.  The mechanisms and processes for such constructions are crucial
to an understanding of their knowledge and consequently for interpreting
the behaviors and actions of a subject.  He proposed that approaches to
the study of such phenomena must be primarily ethnographic, involving
extensive descriptive and interpretive efforts at explaining the complex-
ity.  Cronbach (1975), offering explanations for the absence of strong
experimental evidence of aptitude-treatment interactions in school settings,
recommended a similar methodological shift: researchers reverse the
priority from building generalizations about variables to attending to
each particular situation and the localized effects along with any factors
unique to that locale.  Scriven (1972) also suggested a general relaxation
of the constrained traditional experimentalism.  In examining the tradi-
tional concept of "reliability" he noted that if the usual objections to
self-reports were overcome, educational researchers would naturally pay
more attention to people's reasons for action in contrast to their present
attempts to determine causal accounts of it.

     What this suggests for research on mathematical problem solving must
be decided by the interpretations of scholars in our field.  We do not
lack for ideas and numerous successes in the teaching of a variety of
problematic emphases to differing children.  Perhaps we do need to open
up to new perspectives in the study of problem-solving instruction and
learning with alternative methodologies, such as ethnographic, anthropo-
logic, and even artistic.  We need to shift attention to longitudinal case
studies.  New emphases on situational variables and analysis of environ-
ments should be assumed.  Many scholarly, artistic mathematics educators,
essentially "turned-off" by the Fisherian tradition of experimentalism,
would find new acceptance as researchers.

     The papers of this monograph present differing but compatible
perspectives for investigating mathematical problem solving.  Kilpatrick
offers a careful analysis of variables and methodologies for research on
problem solving.  Adopting the traditional separation of dependent and
independent variables, he presents a thorough typology for studies of
learning and for studies of teaching problem solving.  In his suggestions
for methodologies he also promotes "intensive study of the same classrooms
over an extended period of time."

     Following the theme for the Research Workshop of "instruction in
heuristical methods," Hatfield reviews pedagogical rationales and recent
studies in this area.  Several general qualities of needed research are
offered toward the planning of future investigations.

     Kantowski's paper describes the Soviet "teaching experiment" and its
origin in the U,S.S.R.  Specific suggestions for its potential use in
investigations on mathematical problem solving are presented.  The paper
by Lester offers an extensive review of research efforts directed partic-
ularly at the elementary school levels.  The activities of the tri-site

-------
Mathematical Problem Solving Project, based at Indiana University, are
described as a context for several interesting research results and
questions.  Since the writing of Lester's paper the project has been
discontinued, but several of the project's participants have continued
with their research efforts in this area.

     The Problem Solving Research Workshop served to provide an initial
impetus to the formation of an intellectual consortium of researchers of
mathematical problem solving.  The productive efforts of the participants
during the intervening years has led to solidifying the first stages of
collaborative research.  Optimistically,  new conceptions and results will
be forthcoming.

-------
                           References
Cronbach, L. J.  The logic of experiments on discovery.  In L, S.
     Shulman and E. R. Keisler (eds.), Learning by discovery:  A
     critical appraisal.  Chicago:  Rand McNally, 1966.

Cronbach, L. J.  Beyond the two disciplines of scientific psychology.
     American Psychologist, 1975, _30, 116-127.

Magoon, A. J.  Constructivist approaches in educational research.
     Review of Educational Research, 1977, 47(4), 651-693.

Scriven, M.  Objectivity and subjectivity in educational research.
     In L. G. Thomas (ed.), Philosophical redirection of educational
     research.  Chicago:  National Society for the Study of Education,
     1972.

Shulman, L. S.  Reconstruction of educational research.  Review of
     Educational Research, 1970, 4CI(3), 371-396.

Snow, R. E.  Representative and quasi-representative designs for
     research on teaching.  Review of Educational Research, 1974,
     44, 265-292.

-------
                  Variables and Methodologies in Research
                           on Problem Solving*

                           Jeremy Kilpatrick

                 Teachers College, Columbia University**
     The primary consideration a researcher ought to keep in mind as
he plans and conducts a  study is: What am I trying to find out by doing
this study?  Anyone conducting research on problem solving in mathe-
matics needs to be especially clear about the purpose of a study since
there is so much unexplored  territory in which to get lost.

     Only when one has the purpose clearly formulated is it appropriate
to ask what variables are involved in the study, whether additional
variables should be considered, and what methodology or methodologies
should be used to gather data on these variables.  Although it makes
little sense to choose a variable or methdology before one has settled
on the research question to  be investigated, there may be some value
in discussing the kinds  of variables and methodologies that are avail-
able—as an aid to the researcher who has framed a question but who has
not yet decided how to investigate it.  This paper is an attempt to
survey some variables and methodologies that one might use in research
on problem solving in mathematics, with particular attention to those
that appear most promising.  Research on problem solving per se is con-
sidered separately from  research on the teaching of heuristics.
        Variables in Research on Problem Solving in Mathematics
     Variables can be classified in a variety of ways, depending on
one's purpose.  Classifications include stimulus variables, response
variables, and intervening variables (Travers, 1964), and active
variables versus assigned variables (Ary, Jacobs & Razavieh, 1972).
The most common scheme, borrowed from mathematics and science, is to
classify a variable in a research study as independent or dependent.
In the narrow sense, "independent variable" refers to the condition
manipulated in an experiment.  (One should recall the admonition of
David Hawkins, 1966, that "to call something an independent variable
is not to use a name but to claim an achievement [p. 6].")  In the
broad sense, however, a variable is classed as independent if it is
used in making predictions.  Variables referring to the behavior
being predicted are called dependent variables.  A given variable can
be independent or dependent, depending on its role in the study, but
most variables tend to be used in only one way.
     *Thanks are due to Nick Branca, Sandi Clarkson, Dorothy Goldberg,
Howard Kellogg, Ed Silver, and Phil Smith, who discussed a preliminary
outline of the paper and made many helpful comments and suggestions.
    **Now at the University of Georgia.

-------
                        Independent Variables
     Any study of problem solving in mathematics involves a person
 (subject) solving a mathematical problem (task) under some condition
 (situation).  Each of these components can be used to define a class
of variables.
Subject Variables

     Subject variables can be categorized according to whether or not
they are based on a sample of the subject's behavior.  Some variables
describe the subject as a person:  his sex, height, educational status,
etc.  Such variables can be measured by direct observation, examination
of records, or the subject's own report.  Other variables are based on
inferences derived from a sample of the subject's behavior, say, in
response to a test or questionnaire or as observed by a clinician. Such
variables include various kinds of aptitudes, abilities, attitudes, and
achievement.

     A related, and perhaps more useful, classification of subject
variables is based on the extent to which they can be modified experi-
mentally.  Variables not open to such modification are sometimes termed
"organismic" or "assigned" variables.  Variables open to some modifica-
tion (and requiring a sample of behavior) may be termed "trait" varia-
bles.  (The last two terms are not standard and perhaps not even
satisfactory since such traits as general mental ability are considered
all but unmodifiable in most research contexts, and not all modes of
modification can be termed "instruction."  For the purpose of this
paper, However, the terms will suffice.)

     Organismie variables.  In research on problem solving in mathema-
tics, information on organismic variables such as age, sex, race, and
social class may be gathered to assist in describing the sample, but
with the exception of age and sex, such variables are seldom used as
dependent variables in the design (and then typically in subsidiary
hypotheses only).  Samples of different ages are sometimes drawn in
studies of developmental change, but inferences about the development
of problem-solving proficiency must be especially tentative in view of
the large role instruction apparently plays.  Researchers often must
weigh the advantages of a sample of young, inexperienced subjects (so
that one can study problem-solving processes in a relatively pristine
form, with greater opportunity for detecting "developmental" changes)
against the advantages of a sample of older, more experienced subjects
(so that one can study a greater variety of more sophisticated pro-
cesses).

     Trait variables.  Traits include abilities (such as spatial visual-
ization ability or memory for problems); attitudes, interests, and
values (such as attitude toward mathematics or interest in proving
theorems); and other personality variables relating to perceptual style,

-------
 cognitive style,  self-concept,  persistence,  anxiety, need  for  achieve-
 ment,  sociability,  etc.   Except for  the  variables  dealing  with style,
 which  by  definition refers to a consistency  in behavior  across a wide
 class  of  situations,  each kind  of  trait  variable ranges  from the
 general to the  specific  (as the examples in  parentheses  above  are meant
 to  suggest).  A general  trait such as  persistence,  for example, not
 only is likely  to be less manipulatable  than its more specific cousin
 persistence in  solving problems of type  X, but is  also likely  to be
 less strongly related to ability in  the  processes  used in  solving pro-
 blems  of  type X.  Although ultimately  one wants—for purposes  of a more
 powerful  theory—to link problem-solving process abilities to  variables
 that are  as general as possible, one is  probably best advised  to begin
 with variables  of some specificity.

     As an example,  consider the ability to  estimate.  Psychologists
 tend to think of  this ability—when  they think of  it at  all—as
 general and presumably unitary. A study by  Paull  (1971),  however,
 suggested  not only  that  there are  distinct estimation abilities in
 mathematics but also  that they  may relate in different ways  to
 problem-solving performance.

     Abilities  relating  to memory, classification,  generalization, esti-
 mation, judgment, verification, and  the  like are required  in the solution
 of  a complex mathematical problem  (see Krutetskii,  1976, for a delinea-
 tion of these and other  abilities).  Researchers should  consider
 including  measures  of such abilities in  their studies of problem solving,
 but again  the measures should be specific to the phenomena being studied
 (memory for problems,  say,  as opposed  to associative memory) if a choice
 between general and specific  measures  must be made.

     Trait  variables  that seem  to  have particular  promise  of being asso-
 ciated with problem-solving performance  in mathematics include the
 ability to  generalize a  relationship from a  small  number of  instances,
 the ability to classify  problems according to their mathematical struc-
 ture, the  ability to  recall structural features of  a problem,  the
 ability to  estimate the  magnitude  of a numerical solution, the ability
 to detect  extraneous  and insufficient  data,  a resistance to  fatigue in
 performing  mathematical  tasks,  a sensitivity to problem  situations, a
 preference  for elegance  in  problem solutions,  a reflective cognitive
 style, and  a field independent  cognitive style.  For most of these
variables,  measuring  instruments need  to be  developed and  refined much
 further.    Two categories of trait variables  that might be explored in
 relation to mathematical problem solving are  individual differences in
brain hemisphere functions  (Wittrock,  1974)  and in  ability to  handle
semantic versus syntactic processes  (Simon,  1975).

     instructional history variables.  The instructional history of the
subjects—the topics  they have  studied,  the  problems they have attempted
previously, the techniques  of problem  solving  they have been taught,
the types  of instruction they have received—generates a set of varia-
bles that  can be used  in describing  the  sample of subjects, used in
selecting  the sample,  or  (as  treatment variables—see below) manipulated

-------
                                10

as part of the study.  Such variables are clearly relevant to the
problem-solving process, but they have seldom been considered expli-
citly except in studies comparing the relative effectiveness of two
or more instructional methods.  Failure to take account of variation
in prior instruction may account for some of the failure to find
differences between methods.  Even in studies that do not involve
treatment comparisons, specification of instructional history varia-
bles is likely to assist in the interpretation of results.
Task Variables

     A simple-minded classification of problem tasks (Kilpatrick, 1969)
is according to content and structure.  Both of these categories bear
further examination and elaboration.

     Context variables.  Suppose two mathematical problems involve the
same numbers in the same relation but one deals with rabbits and chickens
in  a  barnyard, the other with two boats on a river.   Most people
would agree that the two problems are the same in (mathematical) struc-
ture, but what word expresses their difference?  "Content" might seem
suitable at first, but on reflection, "context" appears marginally
better since it avoids the connotation that the mathematical content
is different.

     Iftiatever the term, the semantic variables characterizing the
differences between the physical situation modeled in the problem, as
well as the syntactic variables characterizing the language in which
the problem is expressed, need to be explored both analytically and
empirically.

     Structure variables.  The issue of problem structure is also more
complicated than first thought might suggest.  Consider the following
problem:

     Find the volume F of the frustum of a right pyramid with
     square base, given the altitude h of the frustum, the
     length a of a side of its upper base, and the length b of
     a side of its lower base.

One way to characterize the structure of this problem is to say that
the formula

                           a2 + ab + b2
expresses the relation among problem elements and that any other pro-
blem in which elements    ,  ,  , and   are in this relation, with   ,
 , and   given, has the same structure as this problem.  The two
problems might be said to have the same syntactic structure.

-------
                                 11
     Another way  to  characterize  the structure of  the problem is in
 terms of  the network of  all possible steps  from one state of the pro-
 blem to another:   the state-space (Goldin & Luger, 1975).  For the
 problem above,  a  sketch  of the  state-space  is given on the inside
 front cover of  Volume 2  of Mathematical Discovery  (Polya, 1965).
 Another problem having the same state-space might  be said to have the
 same semantic structure.  Or perhaps the distinction is better
 expressed  as the  structure of the problem (in terms of its mathe-
 matical formulation)  versus the structure of the problem space (in
 terms of  the set  of  all  possible  steps in solving  the problem).

     Again, regardless of the terminology adopted, the underlying ideas
 deserve consideration.  A host  of research problems revolve about the
 issue of structure:   Can subjects classify problems according to struc-
 ture?  Is  there an advantage to training subjects  to make such classi-
 fications?  What  problem features facilitate transfer across problems
 differing  in context  but not structure?  Until some dimensions of
 structure  are identified more clearly, the effects of similarities
 and  differences in problem structure cannot be studied systematically.

     Format variables.   A problem may be presented to a subject orally
 or in written form.   It may or  may not involve the manipulation of some
 apparatus.  The instructions may  involve the presentation of rules or
 boundary conditions  to be observed in solving the  problem, or the sub-
 ject may be expected  to  induce  these rules or conditions as part of the
 problem.  The problem may be given all at once or  one part at a time.
 The  subject may or may not be given hints or encouragement as he solves
 the  problem.  He  may  be  asked to  think aloud as he works or to retro-
 spect over the  course of his solution.  He may or may not be permitted or
 be encouraged to  record  scratch work.  All these variables can be
 classed as format  variables.  They are seldom manipulated systemati-
 cally in a study  since they are not ordinarily of  interest to the
mathematics educator.  By ignoring them, the researcher tacitly assumes
 they do not affect the relationships he observes.  Since all generali-
 zations from research on problem  solving need to be validated across
 situations and  data gathering methods, however, variation in problem
 format can be included as part  of  the validation.
Situation Variables

     The dividing line between format variables and situation varia-
bles is not entirely clear.  If a subject were given insufficient or
misleading information about a problem, the issue would appear to be
one of format.  If he were told that the experimenter was interested
in how fast he solved the problem, when in fact the experimenter was
recording how many errors he made, the issue would appear to be one
of situation.  Both cases involve instructions, but it seems useful
to distinguish between variation in the content of the instructions
(format) and variation in the subject's perception of the purpose of
the task (situation).

-------
                                12
     A situation variable concerns the conditions, physical and psycho-
logical, under which the subject solves, or attempts to solve, the
problem.  Situation variables include whether or not the subject volun-
teers for the study, whether or not he is given extrinsic rewards such
as money or grades, whether he works alone or in a group, the time of
day at which data are gathered, the presence or absence of distrac-
tions, the characteristics and behavior of the interviewer, the nature
of the problems given previously and the subject's success with them,
and whether or not the subject was told how he did on the previous pro-
blem.  Like format variables, situation variables themselves are of
relatively little interest to the mathematics educator, although they
may interest the social or educational psychologist.  They are nuisance
variables—since they will not go away, one can only hope they will not
make much difference.  Unfortunately, they may.
                        Dependent Variables
     Some dependent variables are derived from the subject's responses
to a problem task; others require additional samples of behavior.  Let
us consider the latter type first.  (Recall that the studies under
discussion concern problem solving per se; studies involving instruc-
tion in heuristics are yet to be considered.)
Concomitant Variables

     Any of the trait variables mentioned previously may be used as a
dependent variable in a problem-solving study.  For example, one might
investigate how a subject's classification of problems changed after
he had solved a set of problem tasks.  Or one might ask whether his
attitude toward problem solving had changed.

     While working on the problem tasks, the subject may have acquired
new knowledge of or skill in mathematics beyond simply learning how to
solve the problems.  Measures of this knowledge or skill could also
serve as dependent variables.

     As before, one would expect that the more specific the trait,
knowledge, or skill, the greater the likelihood it will be influenced
by the problem tasks.  Experience suggests that one cannot expect much
change in a concomitant variable, however specific, if the number of
tasks is small.
Product Variables

     Product variables are based on dimensions of the subject's solu-
tion to the problem: its correctness, its completeness, its elegance
and economy, the speed with which it was attained, and the number and

-------
                                13
diversity of the alternative solutions the subject finds.  Speed and
correctness are the most commonly used product variables, but others
ought to be considered if possible.
Process Variables

     Process variables are based on the solution path the subject
takes; they are derived from either the subject's verbal report of
his thinking or the manipulations he makes with an apparatus.  Pro-
cess variables relate to such things as the subject's strategy (as
inferred from the sequence of steps he takes), the heuristics he uses,
the algorithms he uses, the efficiency of his solution path, the
extent of his  perseveration in blind alleys, the nature and number
of the errors he makes, and his response to hints.

     Any respectable study of problem solving in mathematics should
include measures of process variables.  Almost nothing is known about
the generalizability of these variables across problems and occasions
although the words "strategy" and "style" connote such generalizabil-
ity (Branca & Kilpatrick, 1972).  Research on subjects' consistency
in their use of process variables would be a valuable contribution.
Evaluation Variables

     It would be nice to have a map of a subject's cognitions after he
has solved a problem.  How does he view the problem?  How does he
relate it to other problems he has solved, other information he
possesses?  Is he aware of the processes he used and the errors he
made?  How confident is he of his solution?  Such questions may be
difficult for subjects to answer directly.  Considerable ingenuity
may be needed in devising instruments to get at the subject's cogni-
tive (and affective) map of the problems he has solved.  A subject's
report of what he was trying to do as he solved a problem and how he
perceives the problem after having worked on it is no less valuable
for being subjective.  His report provides data that can be obtained
in no other way.  There is no particular virtue in labeling such data
as unscientific and ignoring them.
                  Variables in Research on the Teaching
                      of Heuristics in Mathematics
     Up to this point the only "treatment" involved in the research
studies discussed has been the administration of problems to be
solved.  Consequently, the distinction between independent and depen-
dent variables has necessarily been made in the broad sense of
predictor versus behavior predicted rather than the narrow sense of
condition manipulated versus outcome observed.  In studies of the

-------
 teaching of heuristics,  however,  one has full-fledged treatment  varia-
 bles;  namely,  the methods,  materials,  and other  conditions  of instruc-
 tion.   Whether or not  these treatment  variables  are  manipulated
 experimentally, they must be considered in designing such studies.
                        Independent  Variables


      The same  independent  variables considered  earlier can  appear  in
 research on heuristics.  The  subject variables  remain  as  before.   The
 task variables can be  used to characterize problems  used  in instruc-
 tion.   The  situation variables are  essentially  the same,  but the
 researcher  may need to give more  attention to questions of  school
 climate and organization.   (The category of "setting variables" used
 by  Richard  Turner, 1976, in discussing  research on teaching strate-
 gies appears to be roughly equivalent to "situation  variables.")
 Categories  of  variables that  need to be added are instructional treat-
 ment variables, classroom  activity  variables, and teacher variables.


 Instructional  Treatment Variables

      Some variables characterize  an instructional treatment  in general:
 The  extent  to  which the treatment is integrated into ongoing school
 instruction, whether the treatment  is the same  for all subjects or
 individualized, whether the treatment is determined  in advance or
 modified according to  the  subject's response, the extent  to  which the
 treatment tasks resemble the  outcome tasks, etc.  Other variables
 refer to one facet or  another of  the treatment.

     Method variables.  What  heuristics are taught as  part of the treat-
ment?   In what sequence are they  taught?  Is the instruction itself
 heuristic?   (open-ended?   Socratic?   inductive?)  Does the teacher
 illustrate how the heuristics  are used?  Are students  given  names for
 the  heuristics?  What problems  are  used in instruction?  Are problems
 grouped  by type?  Are model problems taught for each type?   What pro-
blems and solutions are discussed with students?  What is the nature
 of this  discussion?  Are students given a chance to discuss  problems
 and  solutions with other students?   Each question implies one or more
variables that might be used  to characterize instructional methods, and
 the  list of such variables  is  limited only by one's ingenuity in asking
such questions.

     As  noted elsewhere (Kilpatrick, 1973), methods-comparison studies
are  typically  flawed in both  conception and execution.  The  common fail-
ure  to define methods operationally  in such studies appears  even more
serious when one considers  the manifold ways in which methods can vary.
Rather than comparing methods,  researchers interested in Instruction in
heuristics should put their energies into devising the best  instruc-
tional program they can and then  demonstrating in detail how the program

-------
                                15
functions and how effective it is in the classroom.   The creation,
tryout, and revision of program components and instruments for measur-
ing effectiveness are research activities of far greater potential
than the comparison of methods.

     Materials variables.  The line between methods and materials is
difficult to draw, but it is probably worthwhile to separate the two
—at least conceptually.  Materials variables in research on heuris-
tics include the nature of the instructional media used, the devices
used to represent problem situations, and the nature of the accompany-
ing prose.  Materials variables in themselves are likely to be of
little interest to mathematics educators.
Classroom Activity Variables

     Instructional treatments ordinarily involve classroom activity.
Some of this activity is in accordance with the researcher's plan,
and as such is part of  the intended treatment.  Much of the activity,
however, is not under the researcher's control.  Although the activity
can ultimately be considered part of the instructional treatment
package—as it works out in practice—a separate category of class-
room activity variables is useful, if only to permit the researcher
to check the variation  in activity within a treatment group and the
congruence between the  actual and the intended treatments.  (In the
latter case, the classroom activity is functioning as a dependent
variable.)

     The Teaching Strategies Project of the Georgia Center for the
Study of Learning and Teaching Mathematics has been examining one
part—albeit an important part—of classroom discourse  (see Cooney,
1976).  Some idea of the broader field of research in classroom acti-
vity is given by Dunkin and Biddle (1974).  Despite the rapid expansion
of the field, apparently only one researcher  (Stilwell, 1967) has done
a descriptive study of  the teaching of problem solving  in mathematics.
Further work along this line is needed.  Although teachers may not  pay
much attention to heuristics during instruction, someone should be  pre-
pared to describe their activity when they do.
Teacher Variables
     Nothing  is more  distressing than to hear researchers  talk of  "the
 teacher variable"—as though there were only one.   If  only there were.
 But  teachers  differ in age,  sex, teaching experience,  self-confidence,
 enthusiasm, philosophy of education,  attitude toward mathematics,  pre-
 ference for unstructured classroom activity, and love  of children.
 They also  differ in problem-solving ability, problem-solving exper-
 ience, knowledge of heuristics,  interest in problem solving, value
 placed on  instruction in heuristics,  willingness to delay  in supplying

-------
                                16
a solution, and ability to accept and to transcend erroneous solutions.
One has only to glance at a list of "teacher competencies" to see the
variety of ways toachers can differ.  Just as no one knows which
teacher competencies, if any, are prerequisite to effective teaching,
so no one knows which teacher variables, if any, might predict the
learning of heuristics.

     The analogous issue of teacher competencies is raised here by
design.  Nothing would be more fruitless than to attempt  a catalog
of teacher variables on the chance that some might prove to be good
predictors of learning.  A much better approach would be to identify
teachers who seemed to have had some success in teaching heuristics,
to see whether this success held up over time and across situations,
and then to explore dimensions of similarity among these teachers and
dimensions of contrast between these teachers and others deemed less
successful.  Only then might one be ready to conjecture some relevant
teacher variables.

     In most research on instruction in heuristics the role of teacher
variables will be either to aid in describing the sample of teachers in
the study or to suggest plausible reasons for the differences likely to
occur in the performance of students taught by different teachers.
Since the sample of teachers is likely to be small, the researcher
should be able to gather considerable information about each teacher,
which would presumably improve either the description or the conjecture.
                       Dependent Variables
     The dependent variables in research on heuristics are the same
dependent variables discussed earlier, plus some new ones.  Classroom
activity variables can be taken as dependent variables (more precisely,
as process variables) if one wishes to learn how the instructional
treatment influenced classroom activity.  Additional dependent varia-
bles that are product variables include all the various measures one
could make of what was learned during instruction.  One of the models
for mathematics achievement (see Wilson, 1971, for examples) might
help in organizing these product variables.
             Methodologies in Research on Problem Solving
              and the Teaching of Heuristics in Mathematics
     The preceding discussion was intended to suggest some variables a
researcher designing a study ought to consider and then either ignore,
eliminate as variables in the design, control through randomization or
matching, or build into the design as independent or dependent varia-
bles.  The effect of the discussion, however, may have been to over-
whelm any reader not already paralyzed by the complexity of empirical
research on problem solving.  Such research is complex, certainly, but

-------
                                17


as long as one keeps coming back to one's research question and asking
what variables and methodologies bear on it, the complexity ought to be
manageable.

     In this paper, methodologies relating to historical research, sur-
vey research, and reviews of the literature are not considered.
Although such studies can be valuable, they demand special methodolo-
gies.  Most of the numerous books on research methods in education
(such as Ary, Jacobs, & Razavieh, 1972; Isaac & Michael, 1971;  Travers,
1964) treat these topics.

     Methodologies in research on problem solving and the teaching of
heuristics in mathematics are so multi-faceted as to defy classifica-
tion.  Consider two of the facets:

     1.  Type of comparison or contrast.  The researcher may be looking
for similarities or differences regarding the same or different sub-
jects' responses to the same or different tasks or treatments on the
same or different occasions or under the same or different conditions.
Each combination of alternatives implies a somewhat different approach.

     2.  Method of gathering data.  The researcher may administer tests
or questionnaires; use an apparatus that presents a problem and either
record the subject's response himself or have it recorded mechanically;
interview subjects given a problem and asked to think aloud or retro-
spect; use personality inventories, protective tests, or such techniques
as word association, the Q-sort, the semantic differential, or the
repertory grid; observe subjects solving problems in the classroom or
elsewhere; make video- or audio-tape recordings of classroom activity;
rely on teachers or students as observers and possibly confederates; act
as a participant observer in a group problem-solving situation; act as
the teacher in an instructional situation, keeping a log and using
recordings to prompt introspection; use the computer to simulate problem-
solving processes from protocols gathered by other means; or use instru-
ments to monitor subjects' physiological processes during problem solving
or instruction.

The Cartesian product of all possible types of comparison or contrast
with all combinations of data-gathering methods only begins to suggest
the variety of methodologies one could employ.

     The most promising methodologies for research on problem solving
in mathematics are those involving intensive study of the same set of
subjects over an extended period of time.  The subjects must solve a
large number of problems of diverse types in order to permit confident
generalizations about the processes they use.  Numerous measures of
trait variables should be obtained, and control should be exercised
over instructional history variables.  Case studies of subjects selected
because of notable giftedness in mathematics or notable difficulty with
mathematics may be particularly useful.  Cross-age studies of develop-
mental trends in problem solving may help to suggest process variables
that should be studied further, but longitudinal studies are obviously
to be preferred.

-------
                                 18
      The most promising methodologies for research on the teaching of
heuristics in mathematics are those involving intensive study of the
same classrooms over an extended period of time.  Of special interest
are designs in which the experimenter works with the teacher during
the course of an academic term or so, observing the effects of various
modifications in instruction and using interviews with students to
supplement test and observational data.  Such studies should lead to
the development of materials for instruction in heuristics.  The class-
room activities of teachers identified as especially effective in
teaching problem-solving techniques should be analyzed and contrasted
with the classroom activities of teachers having more ordinary attain-
ments.  Studies in which the instruction is programmed to control
sources of teacher variation may help to suggest which heuristics are
most teachable, but studies involving at least some instruction by
teachers should predominate.

      Experimental studies in which all variables are under tight
control are not likely to be of much value in the present state of
our ignorance as to how people solve complex mathematical problems
and how they might be led to use heuristic methods.  Too much develop-
mental work is needed before experimentation could be effective.  For
example, instruments and techniques must be developed and validated
for assessing most of the variables discussed in this paper.

      No one is suggesting that researchers abandon the designs and
techniques that have served so well in empirical research.  But a
broader conception of research is needed, and an openness to new
techniques, if studies of problem-solving processes and the teaching
of heuristics are to have an impact.

      Some years ago a group of researchers gave a battery of psycho-
logical tests each summer to mathematically talented senior high school
students attending special summer institutes at Florida State Univer-
sity.  The scores on the tests were intercorrelated, and some
correlation coefficients were significant, some not.  Several research
reports were published (Kennedy, 1962; Kennedy, Cottrell & Smith, 1963,
1964; Kennedy and the Human Development Staff, 1960; Kennedy, Nelson,
Lindner, Turner & Moon, 1960).  As Krutetskii (1976) notes, the process
of solution did not appear to interest the researchers—yet what rich
material could have been obtained from these gifted students if one
were to study their thinking processes in dealing with mathematical
problems.  Why were the students simply given a battery of tests to
take instead of being asked to solve mathematical problems?  It's a
good question.

-------
                                   19
                               References
Ary, D.; Jacobs, L. C.; and Razavieh, A.  Introduction to research in
      _gducat:ign.  New York:  Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1972.

Branca, N. A. and Kilpatrick, J.  The consistency of strategies in the
      learning of mathematical structures.  Journal for Research in
      Mathematics Education, 1972, 3^, 132-140.

Cooney, T. J. (Ed.).  Teaching strategies:  Papers from a jresearch work-
      shop.  Columbus, Ohio:  ERIC/SMEAC, 1976.

Dunkin, M. J. and Biddle, B. J.  The study of teaching.  New York:  Holt,
      Rinehart & Winston, 1974.

Goldin, G. A. and Luger, G. F.  Implications of problem structure for
      teaching problem-solving heuristics.  Paper presented at the
      meeting of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics,
      Denver, April 1975.

Hawkins, D.  Learning the unteachable.  In L. S. Shulman and E. R.
      Keislar (Eds.), Learning by discovery^  A critical appraisal.
      Chicago:  Rand McNally, 1966.

Isaac, S. and Michael, W. B.  Handbook  in research and evaluation.  San
      Diego:  Knapp, 1971.

Kennedy, W. A.  MMPI profiles of gifted adolescents.  Journal of Clini-
      cal Psychology. 1962, 18, 148-149.

Kennedy, W. A.; Cottrell, T.; and Smith, A.  Norms of gifted adolescents
      on the Rotter Incomplete Sentence Blank,  Journal of Clinical
      Psychology. 1963, 19, 314-315.

Kennedy, W. A.; Cottrell, T.; and Smith, A.  EPPS norms for mathemati-
      cally gifted adolescents.  Psychological Reports, 1964, _14_, 342.

Kennedy, W. A. and the Human Development Clinic Staff,  A multidimen-
      sional study of mathematically gifted adolescents.  Child
      Development, 1960, 31, 655-666.

Kennedy, W. A.; Nelson, W.; Lindner, R.; Turner, J. ;  and Moon, H.
      Psychological measurements of  future scientists   Psychological
      Reports, 1960, 7_, 515-517.

Kilpatrick, J.  Problem solving in mathematics.  Review of Educational
      Research, 1969, _3J9, 523-534.

Kilpatrick, J,  Research in the teaching and learning of mathematics.
      Paper presented at the meeting of the Mathematical Association
      of America, Dallas, January 1973,

-------
                                20


Krutetskii, V. A.  The psychology of mathematical abilities in school
      children.  Chicago :  University of Chicago Press, 1976.

Paull, D. R.  The ability to estimate in mathematics.  Unpublished
      doctoral dissertation, Teachers College, Columbia University,
      1971.

Polya, G.  Mathematical discovery;  On understanding, learning, and
      teaching problem solving, Vol. 2.  New York:  Wiley, 1965.

Simon, H. A.  Knowledge and skill in problem solving.  Invited address
      presented at the meeting of the Special Interest Group for
      Research in Mathematics Education, American Educational Research
      Association, Washington, March 1975.
Stilwell, M. E.  The development ^gncL analysis of a category system for
      systematic observation of teacher-pupil interaction during geo-
      metry problem-solving activity.   (Doctoral dissertation, Cornell
      University)  Ann Arbor, Michigan:  University Microfilms, 1968,
      No. 68-893.

Travers, R. M. W.  An introduction to educational research.  (2nd ed.)
      New York:  Macmillan, 1964.

Turner, R. L.  Design problems in research on teaching strategies in
      mathematics.  In T. J. Cooney (Ed.), Teaching strategies;  Papers
      from a research workshop.  Columbus, Ohio:  ERIC/SMEAC, 1976.

Wilson, J. W.  Evaluation of learning in secondary school mathematics.
      In B. S. Bloom, J. T. Hastings,  and G. F. Madaus (Eds.), Hand-
      book on formative and summative evaluation of student learning.
      New York:  McGraw-Hill, 1971.

Wittrock, M. C.  A generative model of mathematics learning.  Journal
      for Research in Mathematics Education, 1974, _5» 181-196.

-------
                                   21
        Heuristical Emphases in the Instruction of  Mathematical
               Problem Solving:  Rationales and Research


                           Larry L. HatfieId

                         University of Georgia
     The significance of the goal of improving the learner's prohlem-
solving competence within school mathematics is well established.
Recommendations for emphasizing problem-solving techniques in teaching
mathematics can be found throughout the history of mathematics educa-
tion (Jones, 1970).  However, in spite of an implored need that
educators must know much more about using problems to stimulate inde-
pendent and creative thinking, the teaching and learning of problem
solving has only occasionally been investigated by mathematics educa-
tion researchers.

     The general goal of the Problem-Solving Project of the Georgia
Center for the Study of Learning and Teaching Mathematics (GCSLTM) is
to organize and conduct coordinated studies of the conditions for and
the effects of learning and instruction of mathematics which emphasizes
problem solving.  Several distinct areas of study encompassing this
goal can be identified.  These areas include:

     (a)  studies devoted to identification of strategies and pro-
          cesses used in solving various mathematical problems,
          including a search for aptitudes related to these strate-
          gies and processes;

     (b)  studies devoted to development of clinical procedures  for
          observing and analyzing mathematical problem-solving
          behaviors;

     (c)  studies devoted to development of instructional procedures
          aimed at improving a student's problem-solving capabili-
          ties;

     (d)  studies devoted to development of teacher  training proce-
          dures  to result in deliberate employment of instructional
          methods aimed at  enhancing  the growth of problem-solving
          capabilities of students;

     (e)  studies of  an expository  nature,  including analytical
          developments as well as technical reports  and interpre-
          tative reports of activities and  findings  of this problem-
          solving research  and development.

-------
                                 22
      The  specific  project goals during the  first year are:  (a) to
conduct various investigations in mathematical problem solving, (b)
to provide an organized synthesis of the literature in mathematical
problem solving appropriate to the investigations underway and to
the interests of the investigators, (c) to  identify detailed speci-
fications for studies to be undertaken in the second round of the
GCSLTM's activities, and (d) to establish a working consortia model
for conducting coordinated series of investigations.  The conduct of
the Problem-Solving Research Workshop represents a major step toward
obtaining these first-year goals.

     The research on problem-solving behavior to be found in the
literature of psychology and education (including mathematical educa-
tion) is considerably varied.  As a matter of project strategy it was
decided that the theme of the Problem-Solving Workshop would be
"instruction in heuristical methods."  While intending to provide a
focus and a direction for our initial research efforts particularly
into area c (development of instructional procedures), such an empha-
sis should eventually span the five areas for studies noted earlier.

     The purpose of this paper is to contribute a common perspective
for conceptualizing investigations reflecting the theme of teaching
mathematical problem solving by, and for, heuristical methods.  The
following sections include a discussion of rationales for emphasiz-
ing heuristical precepts in teaching and researching mathematical
problem solving, a review of selected recent mathematical education
research involving heuristical methods, and an identification of
possible directions and dimensions of studies to be undertaken in
the Problem-Solving Project.
         Instruction in Heuristical Methods:  What, Why and How
     The most widely known contemporary and practical treatment of
heuristic is due to the eminent mathematician Polya (1957, 1962,
1965).  Recently Wickelgren (1974) has sought to provide an extension
of such treatments, blending significant developments from the field
of artificial intelligence and information processing models with
Polya's maxims.  In addition, Higgins (1971) offers an interpretation
of heuristic as it applies to a methodology for mathematics instruc-
tion termed "heuristic teaching."  It must be assumed that Workshop
participant- are reasonably familiar with these elaborate writings.
At the same time a brief review of particular aspects will serve to
highlight and clarify certain points of view.

     What are "heuristical methods" for problem solving?  A synopsis
from Polya's discussions is presented in search of clarification.

     Heuristic, or heuretic, or "ars inveniendi" was the name of
     a certain branch of study, not very clearly circumscribed,
     belonging to logic, or to philosophy, or to psychology, often

-------
                               23
     outlined,  seldom presented  in detail, and as good as for-
     gotten today.   The aim of heuristic  is  to study  the methods
     and rules  of discovery and  invention.   (Polya, 1957, p. 112)

     Heuristic,  as an adjective,  means  "serving  to discover,"
     (Polya,  1957,  p. 113)

     Modern heuristic endeavors  to understand the process of
     solving problems especially the mental  operations typically
     useful in  this process,  It has various sources  of informa-
     tion none  of which should be neglected,  A  serious study of
     heuristic  should take into  account both the logical and
     psychological background,  it should  not neglect  what such
     older writers as Pappus, Descartes,  Leibnitz and Bolzano
     have to say about the subject,  but it should least neglect
     unbiased experience.  Experience in  solving problems and
     experience in watching other people  solving problems must
     be the basis on which heuristic is built.  (Polya, 1957,
     pp. 129-30)

     Heuristic  reasoning is reasoning not regarded  as final and
     strict but as provisional and  plausible only, whose  purpose
     is to discover the solution of the present  problem.  We  are
     often obliged to use heuristic reasoning.  We  shall  attain
     complete certainty when we shall have obtained the  complete
     solution,  but before obtaining certainty we must often be
     satisfied with a more or less  plausible guess,   CPolya,  1957,
     p. 113)

     Polya approaches heuristic from a practical teacher~oriented
aspect:  "I am trying, by all the means at my disposal,  to  entice
the reader to do problems and to think about the means and  methods
he uses in doing them" CPolya, 1362, p, vi).  His  detailed  "case
histories" of problem solutions feature questions  and suggestions
which, organized into four phases of work on the solution (under-
standing, planning, carrying out, looking back), have come  to be
known as his "planning heuristic,"  According to Polya,  these ques-
tions and suggestions have two common characteristics,  generality
(in that they indicate a general direction of action and thus may
help unobtrusively) and common sense (in order that they can occur
naturally or easily to the solver himself).   But the significant
assumption by Polya is the following:

     If the reader is sufficiently acquainted with the list and
     can see, behind the suggestion, the action suggested,  he
     may realize that the list enumerates, indirectly, mental
     operations typically useful for the solution of problems.
     (Polya, 1957, p. 2)

-------
                               24
     Thus, "heuristical methods" for solving problems include plaus-
ible but uncertain actions of a general yet natural character.  To
know and to use "heuristical methods" at some level of effective-
ness suggests that one also knows in some fashion the cognitive
operations bearing on one's own problem-solving behavior.

     This notion hints at potentially important, yet typically
subtle, distinctions:  curriculum or instruction aimed at teaching
for problem solving versus teaching about problem solving versus
teaching via problem solving.  Most contemporary school mathematics
textbooks claim to teach for problem-solving outcomes; in the sense
that organized, usable knowledge in mathematics (concepts, princi-
ples, skills) is essential to being an effective problem solver in
mathematics, these claims are partially justified.  Few texts, how-
ever, seek to teach rather explicitly about problem solving in the
sense of heuristic.  And correspondingly few textbooks approach
content as heuristicnlly oriented problem solving as Polya or
Higgins recommend.

     What should constitute "instruction in heuristical methods,"
particularly as it relates to mathematical education, is still an
ill-defined matter.  Again, one can find various characterizations
and exemplifications offered by Polya  (1962, 1965), Wickelgren
(1974), Covington and Crutchfield (1965), Wilson  (1967), Butts
(1973), and others.  An essential feature seems to be the explicit
identification and use of heuristical  ploys within the act of
solving mathematical problems.  Sometimes these are modeled by the
teacher or instructional materials to  be observed and to be imitated
by the learner, while at other  times they are to be  initiated and to
be practiced in the  learner's own problem-solving actions.  Further
discussion on this question will be offered in  the final section of
this paper relating  particular  strategies for our research.

     Why  should mathematics  educators  choose to give "instruction
in heuristical methods" of problem solving?  Here again,  the  cogent
arguments offered by Polya  (see especially 1957,  pp. 1-32; 1965,
pp. 99-142) encompass learner motivation, educational relevancy,
general culture, enhancing common sense  reasoning, and active
learning.  These arguments must certainly be accompanied  now  by
acceptable research  findings regarding the efficacy  of learning
and using heuristical percepts  for  improved problem-solving  capa-
city.  Herein  lies a central point  of  departure for  the investiga-
tions  to  be  stimulated  in  this  Project.

-------
                             25

                                                              1
       Selected Investigatjjms Emphasizing Heuristical Methods
     Recently several investigations of mathematical problem solving
have attempted to teach explicitly heuristical precepts or with heur-
istical methods or to analyze problem-solving protocols using systems
based on Polya's ideas.  A number of studies have used various treat-
ments of task-specific and general "heuristics" in attempting to
improve subjects' problem-solving competence through instruction,
Ashton (1962) gave ten weeks of heuristic-oriented instruction based
on Polya's work to ninth grade algebra students.  These students,
when compared with a control group receiving conventional instruction,
were better able to solve word problems,

     Covington and Crutchfield (1965) also obtained superiority of
the instructed children in measures of divergent thinking, original-
ity, and perceived value of problem solving.  These subjects used
comic book format to engage students in developing "heuristics" for
non-mathematical problems.  Olton  (1967) conducted an extensive test
of the revised version of this program and confirmed the positive
effects on a diverse set of performance indicators.  His students
achieved up to 50 percent higher scores on post-test measures where
the teacher discussed each lesson, a finding which seems to support
the value Polya has assigned to a  "looking back" phase  in his
"planning heuristic."  Jerman  (1971) used The Productive Thinking
Program and a Modified Wanted-Given Program  (after Wilson, 1967)
with fifth grade students and  concluded that teaching problem solving
in mathematics to students of  this age can best be done  in a mathe-
matical context using a wanted-given approach, whereas  either system-
atic approach to problem solving was more effective than not providing
any systematic instruction,

     Wilson  (1967) and Smith  (1973)  compared subjects  taught mathema-
tical problem solving using either "task-specific heuristics" or
"general heuristics" via self-instructional  booklets.   In  each  study
the dependent variables were  time  measures and number  of correct  steps
for each section of  the criterion  test.  Both  investigators hypothe-
sized that task-specific heuristical  instruction would  lead to
superior performances on the  training  tasks  but poorer  performances
on  the  transfer  tasks than would  instruction in the use of general
heuristics.  Analysis of performances  of Wilson's subjects on  the
training tasks and five transfer  tasks revealed that  only  the  general
 (planning) heuristic was superior  to  the others  (a  task-specific  heur-
istic applicable only  to  the  topic under study  and  a  means-end
heuristic):  it is  suggested that  general heuristics learned  in  the
first training task  were practiced on  the  second  task,  thereby  facil-
itating transfer.  Thus, Wilson  failed  to  confirm his central  hypo-
thesis  and,  in fact,  found  that  one  of his general  heuristics  (the
       The reader is urged to examine other more comprehensive reviews
 of problem-solving research, including Kilpatrick (1969),  Riedesel
 (1969),  Simon and Newell (1971),  and Suydatn (1972).

-------
                                26
 planning heuristic)  led to better performance on a  training  task than
 did the heuristic specific to  that task.   One possible  explanation
 lies in the  potential power of a  heuristical  maxim:   the  availability
 of the maxim to the  problem solver and  his efficiency in  using  the
 advice.  In  part, the problem  solver must  recognize  that  general
 heuristics are  indeed general  and therefore possibly applicable to
 solving an unfamiliar problem  wherein known task-specific strategies
 are not useful.   The limited learning time (two  3-hour  sessions)  and
 number of problem-solving  episodes (about  20  problems)  may have
 resulted in  less  practicing of general  heuristics than  may be neces-
 sary for subjects to become operable.

      Smith's (1973)  study,  patterned after Wilson's,  attempted  to
 emphasize the generality of its general heuristics  to a greater
 extent than  Wilson did with his.   Subjects were  also given more
 opportunity  to  practice similar heuristic  procedures  in a variety
 of settings.  The data for  Smith's subjects failed to support the
 hypothesis that instruction in heuristics  differing  in  level of
 generality leads  to  differences in performance on transfer tasks.
 Questionnaires  and interviews  suggested that, while  subjects claimed
 to have used the  heuristics when  completing a given  learning task,
 almost  no subjects attempted to use the general  heuristic  on the
 transfer tasks.   Smith conjectured  the  possible  reasons for the
 apparent  abandonment  of the planning heuristic on these unfamiliar
 tasks.   Often the more general heuristic does not always  suggest
 itself;  the problem  solver  must often make  a conscious  effort to
 reach  into his  "bag  of heuristics" when he  is stymied.  Besides know-
 ing  strategies which  he believes will be of use, the  subject must
 think  of  trying them  when he gets  stuck.  Adequate practice and
 encouragement,  couched in successful applications of  heuristical
 advice,  seem absolutely essential  for true operationality  of such
 strategies.   In positing suggestions for further study,  Smith notes:

     Investigators of human  problem solving are probably well
     advised to incorporate  some means  of studying a  subject's
     behavior in  addition to examining  test or time scores.
     Selected interviews or  problem-solving questionnaires are
     one  possibility.   Some  form of protocol analysis might
     provide valuable  insights into the apparent lack of trans-
     fer  power of  general heuristic advice.  In fact, exploratory
     studies might be more valuable than experimental ones, given
     the present  state of our knowledge about human problem solv-
     ing.  A researcher who devotes his  full energy to studying
     the problem-solving processes of his subjects rather than
     the products  they produce may discover revealing behavior
     patterns.   (pp.   100-1),

     Kilpatrick (1967), using a system based on heuristical processes
identified by Polya,  analyzed problem-solving protocols  of 56 junior
high school  students   in relation to their performances on a battery
of aptitude,  achievement,  and attitude scales.  The processes used in

-------
                                27

solving word problems seemed unrelated to observed systematic styles
of approach to spatial and numerical tasks.  Subjects who attempted
to set up equations Cpre-algebra students) were significantly super-
ior to others on measures of quantitative ability, mathematics
achievement, word fluency, general reasoning, logical reasoning, and
a reflective conceptual tempo.  Those subjects who used the most
trial and error were higher than the others in quantitative ability
and mathematics achievement.  Those subjects who used the least trial
and error had the most trouble with the word problems, spent the least
time on them, and got the fewest number correct.

     Lucas (1972) and Goldberg (1973) employed a modified version of
Kilpatrick's (1967) system for coding and analyzing the problem-
solving protocols of subjects following the application of instruc-
tional treatments designed to exhibit and use a general planning
heuristic.  Lucas made an exploratory study of the effects of teaching
heuristic in calculus.  Following an eight-week instructional period
during which one class was taught using the style of teaching suggested
by Polya along with prepared materials that defined and demonstrated
the use of heuristical advice while another class learned calculus
without explicit attention to heuristics, volunteers from both classes
were interviewed individually and asked to solve problems while think-
ing aloud.  The protocols of these thirty subjects were analyzed to
identify strategies.

     Lucas was able to isolate thirty-eight variables that represented
heuristical ideas, indicators of difficulty, types of errors, and per-
formance measures of time and product score.  He found evidence of
differences in performance between subjects in the heuristical and
non-heuristical treatments, but his conclusions were stated tentatively,
reflecting the purpose of identifying behavioral variables rather than
hypothesis testing.  Subjects from the heuristical treatment were judged
superior in their ability to solve problems when the criterion was score
on approach, on plan, or on result; they read the problems more easily
and hesitated less as they worked.  Some heuristics were used more fre-
quently by subjects taught heuristically:  working backward, using the
result or method of a related problem, and introducing suitable nota-
tion.  Heuristical training did not appear to affect the use of
diagrams, the use of trial and error, the number of errors, the number
of aborted solutions, or the ability to write proofs.  Lucas concluded
that Kilpatrick's system can be used to characterize college students'
problem-solving behavior, and that heuristical maxims can be taught in
calculus without infringing on course content.  That heuristical
instruction was no more time consuming than the kind of instruction
given to the control class may be explained because the written
instruction in heuristics was reserved for extra-class time.  Larsen
(1960) had found that calculus students instructed in heuristical ideas
learned them, but apparently at the expense of normal course content.

     Goldberg (1973) examined the effects of instruction in heuristical
advice for writing proofs, suggested by Polya, on the ability of college
students not majoring in mathematics to construct proofs in number
theory.  Two sets of seven self-instructional booklets written by the

-------
                                28
 investigator were used with 238 subjects enrolled  in nine classes.
 Classes were randomly assigned to one of three different treatments:
 reinforced heuristic, unreinforced heuristic, and  non-heuristic.
 Following the six-week experimental period, measures of understanding
 of number theory concepts, ability to construct proofs, attitude
 toward the self-instructional booklets, and attitude toward problem
 solving were obtained.  The analyses indicated that heuristic instruc-
 tion with reinforcement in class is relatively more beneficial than
 unreinforced or non-heuristic instruction with respect to understand-
 ing number theory concepts and writing proofs for  high ability
 students.  This result was also found favoring unreinforced heuristic
 instruction over instruction by imitating examples (non-heuristic).
 The non-heuristic self-instructional booklets were found more helpful,
 easier, and generally more appealing than the heuristic self-
 instructional booklets.  Goldberg suggests that the non-heuristic
 materials were less threatening and more fun due to the inclusion
 of puzzles and number tricks whose purpose was to help to make the
 time to complete the booklets more comparable to the heuristical
 treatment.

     A more important effect was observed in the more positive atti-
 tudes toward the problem-solving process of the subjects in the
 reinforced heuristic treatment than students whose instruction in
 heuristics was not reinforced in class.  Applying a coding system to
 the written proofs of students scoring in the top  third of the proofs
 posttest, she found that students given reinforced heuristic instruc-
 tion used heuristics more than students who had studied number theory
 by imitating examples.  Among the precepts noted, these students wrote
 "given" or "prove," rephrased the conclusion of the problem, used
 theorems more frequently than definitions, introduced suitable nota-
 tion, and worked backwards in their proofs more frequently than
 proficient proof writers who were not given reinforced heuristic
 instruction.

     Vos (1976)  compared the effects of three instructional strategies
 on problem-solving behaviors in secondary school mathematics.  Five
 particular behaviors (drawing a diagram, approximating and verifying,
 constructing an algebraic equation, classifying data, and constructing
 a chart) were identified as problem-solving behaviors or "heuristics."
 Each of three experimental treatments,  classified as Repetition (R),
 List (L), and Behavior Instruction (B), involved an exposure to twenty
mathematical problems but with variations in the placement of, and
 emphasis on, one of the five implied problem-solving behaviors.  Simply
 stated, the treatments were:   R presented the problem task only;  L
 presented the problem task which was momentarily interrupted with a
 checklist of desirable problem-solving behaviors and individual written
 instruction in a specific useful solving behavior followed by a return
 to the problem task; and B initially presented individual written
 instruction in a specific problem-solving behavior followed by the
 same training problem task used in L and R.   Each problem task adminis-
 tered through self-instructional materials took about twenty minutes to
 complete.  Subjects in six mathematics  classes (grades 9, 10, 11)  at a

-------
                                29
private Iowa school were randomly assigned within classes to one of
the three treatments which occurred over about fifteen weeks.  Post-
treatment data included scores from a Problem-Solving Approach Test
(PSAT) and a Problem-Solving Test (PST).  PSAT consisted of two parts
each having problem statements with choices indicating an approach
that could best be used to solve the problem.  Part I offered choices
directly related to the five instructed problem-solving behaviors
whereas Part II sought to measure transfer in using other problem-
solving behaviors.  The PST consisted of seven problems seeking a free
written response with encouragement to write all their thoughts about
the situation.  In summarizing the results found from the various data
analyses, Vos asserts that specific instruction in utilizing problem-
solving behaviors increased the effective use of the behaviors.  The
evidence supports the idea that classroom mathematics instruction
should involve specific instruction in a set of problem-solving
behaviors.

     Webb (1975) studied the problem-solving processes used by forty
second year high school algebra students while solving eight problems.
The aims of his study were (a) to consider how cognitive and affec-
tive variables and the use of heuristical strategies are related to
each other and to the ability of high school students to solve pro-
blems, (b) to identify problem-specific strategies from those used in
solving problems in general, and (c) to identify problem-solving modes
of groups of students.  Protocols from individual interviews were
analyzed using a coding system adapted from Kilpatrick (1967) and Lucas
(1972).  Data from sixteen cognitive and affective pretests, frequen-
cies of problem-solving processes, and total problem inventory scores
were analyzed using principal component, regression and cluster analy-
ses.

     Webb observed that mathematics achievement was the variable with
the highest relation to mathematical problem-solving ability.  The use
of heuristical strategies had some relation to mathematical problem-
solving ability not accounted for by mathematics achievement.  In
particular, the component Pictorial Representation accounted for a
significant amount of the variance.  Thus, the processes used by
students in this study added to their ability to solve problems beyond
their mathematical conceptual knowledge (mathematics achievement).

     Furthermore, Webb noted that students who used a wide range of
heuristical strategies, on the average, were better problem solvers.
Most of these heuristical strategies were found to be problem-specific.
This implies that in order to solve several different problems, a
range of problem-specific strategies needs to be employed.  Strategies
such as "specialization" and "successive approximations" were used at
least once on six of the eight problems, but were used by more students
on one or two of the problems.  One possible reason for the restricted
use of these strategies is that students only used the strategies in
obvious ways and did not employ the strategies where they could be
strategically used.  Webb suggests that one direction for research
would be to examine whether students can be taught to use such strate-
gies to solve a wide range of problems.

-------
                                30
     Particular combinations of strategies appeared to relate to per-
formance.  Students who used a moderate amount of trial-and-error and
a moderate amount of equations or who use equations often and trial-
and-error seldom performed about the same on the Problem-Solving
Inventory.  Students who used a high frequency of trial-and-error and
had a low use of equations did not do as well.  These results are
somewhat counter to Kilpatrick's (1967) observations of the relative
effectiveness of trial-and-error methods.  However, for these high
school students and for the problems in the inventory, it appeared
that trial-and-error as an approach to problem solving had a value
as a supplementary process to the use of equations, but not as a
replacement for the use of equations.

     Kantowski (1974) conducted a "teaching experiment" (quite in the
Soviet sense) as a clinical exploratory study of processes used by
eight grade nine subjects in solving non-routine "to show" problems
in geometry.  She noted Talyzina's (1970) observation that subjects
who were successful problem solvers in geometry introduced an "opera-
tive proposition" ("heuristic") into the solution.  Kantowski observed
that if the "heuristics" used by her students were goal-orj^ented (that
is, specifically related to the conclusions of the problem) the solu-
tion tended to be more efficient.  She observed a dramatic increase
in the use of goal-oriented "heuristics" across her instructional
treatments on "heuristics" and geometry problem solving.

     But she made an even more penetrating observation.  Of course,
valid deductions are often essential to solving mathematics problems.
Such deductions are commonly made by analysis and synthesis.  She took
analysis to be what Polya refers to as "decomposing" or making infer-
ences from what is known.  Synthesis, on the other hand, is a "recom-
bining" of facts to form a new whole.  She examined where these
analytic and synthetic deductions occurred in the sequence of processes
during solution and their relationship to the use of "heuristics."  The
manifestation of regular patterns of analysis and synthesis among suc-
cessful problem solvers is striking.  In a high percentage of cases
these regular patterns of analysis and synthesis were immediately
preceded by a goal-oriented "heuristic."  In cases where non-goal-
oriented "heuristics" were introduced, the patterns of analysis and
synthesis were generally irregular and superfluous.

     McClintock (1975) reported the relative effects of verbalization
of "heuristics" on transfer of learning.  In three one-hour periods
of instruction (methods were discovery, expository or control) general-
izations for the sums, sums of squares, and sums of cubes of the first
n natural numbers were taught (modes were teacher verbal instruction,
self-instructional reading, or combined teacher verbal and self-
instructional reading).  Following initial learning students attempted
to two-item practice test whereupon a "heuristics verbalization" group
responded verbally to the request to state what they recalled having
done in solving the problems with encouragement to use Polya's four

-------
                                31
phases for their description.  Thereafter, all groups were given trans-
fer tasks (12 problems to solve) related to the taught generalizations.
Significant effects for method of instruction, "heuristics verbaliza-
tion," and interactions were found.  It appeared that a combined
instructional mode followed by the "looking back" of the verbalization
of "heuristics" tended to produce greater transfer to problem-solving
tasks.
"Looking Back" at These Studies

     What do these few premier investigations offer as results or
directions for future research?  What features seem common among these
studies?  Are there research methodologies, variables or designs appear-
ing to encompass more salient aspects of the behaviors we should or
might be studying?

     Defining constructs.  The plague of ill-defined constructs which
permeates much of educational research is manifested in most of these
investigations.  The broad implications of the notion heuristic result
in its usage being at best varied but more often unclear.  In one
sense it involves the "science" (or art) of studying and describing
the mental processes or operations of solving problems.  This would
seem to connote an applied epistemology:  The study of, and use of,
knowledge itself.  Despite its focus on mathematical problem solving
serving to provide considerable clarification, Polya's more practical
approach does not relay a cognitive psychological theory of the nature
and usage of heuristic.

     The studies described earlier generally use the term "heuristics"
(plural) in the sense suggested by Kilpatrick:  ". . .as any device
technique, rule of thumb, etc., that improves problem-solving perfor-
mance" (1967, p. 19).  The maxims, questions, precepts, ploys, and
suggestions offered by Polya and others are most often referred to as
"heuristics."  Usually no more than implicitly are the actual behaviors
of the problem-solving act considered.  Particularly missing are dis-
cussions of the mental operations and cognitive structures which are
(a) necessary to assimilate the heuristical precept as a potentially
powerful item of knowledge for future problem solving, (b) required
to recall and then apply the heuristical advice, (c) "triggered" by
the recall or suggestion of an heuristical statement.

     Finally there is further confusion resulting from the use of
heuristical notions in conceptualizing instructional methods for
teaching about the maxims and their use in problem solving.  It would
seem that one could teach heuristical advice as generalizations via
expository instruction.  Yet Polya implores the teacher to use a
pedagogy of problem setting, teacher questioning, active learner
participation and choice, and post-solution discussions that reflect
the techniques of an heuristical problem-solving approach.

-------
                                32
     Another notion requiring further clarification is problem-solving
strategy.  Various references can be found to a "trial and error
strategy," an "inductive strategy," "heuristic strategy," "information
processing strategy," an "algorithmic strategy," or a "strategy of
indirect proof."  The considerable range of interpretation occurring in
these examples illustrates the confusion.  Yet the intuitive idea of
strategy has appeal as a useful idea for describing or contrasting
approaches to a problem solution,

     Studying processes.  Surprisingly,  few of these studies used as
dependent measures the actual process sequence trace of the subject's
problem-solving act.  Rather time to solution, number of correct steps,
and correct problem answers are used.  Kilpatrick, Lucas, Kantowski,
and Webb interviewed subjects to obtain protocol analyses of the pro-
cess sequences.  Yet even here the primary dependent measures became
type and frequency of occurrence of various heuristical precepts. Only
Kantowski reports identified patterns in the sequences noted for her
subjects.

     The particular manifestation of student behaviors, including
explicit as well as apparent use of heuristical ploys, may be a way
to characterize solution strategies.  Similarities across problems
(both in content and sequence) of processes used by a subject could
be described as a solution strategy known to that problem solver.
Or consistent displays of process sequences across subjects for a
particular problem or type of problem could empirically describe the
solution strategies commonly associated with that problem.

     The protocol analyses undertaken in some of these studies engaged
the seminal coding system, or variations thereof, provided by Kil-
patrick  (1967).  Obviously the nature and quality of process analyses
will be  contingent upon the sensitivity and comprehensiveness of the
interview and the template of the coding scheme subsequently used on
the protocols.

     Generality of heuristics.  Several of these investigations dealt
with a "generality of heuristics" dimension.  Recall Polya's recogni-
tion of  the generality characteristic of his questions and suggestions.
As a teacher, he seeks to offer advice as unobtrusively as possible so
that steps in the emerging solution path do not become too obvious to
the solver.  At the same time he wants the advice to be widely appli-
cable to many different problems.  Any research findings about "general
vs. task-specific" heuristics are very tentative.  Most investigations
were able to observe positive effects from subjects being taught or
using more general heuristical precepts.  Interestingly, few of  these
studies  offer direct evidence regarding whether the subjects indeed
learned  the taught "heuristics."  Most studies employed transfer tasks
to observe knowledge of heuristical advice by observing its use  in
problem  solving.

-------
                                33
     Instructional intervention.  Following the artistic lead offered
by Polya, many of these investigators have sought to Improve the pro-
blem solving performances of their subjects by directly providing
special instruction in "heuristics."  Programmed instruction booklets
were used in several studies.  Some investigators incorporated especial
concern for accompanying such booklets with teacher reinforcement (dis-
cussion) of the "heuristics" employed.  Yet the results of the effects
of explicitly developing a "looking back" phase in the instruction are
mixed.

     The characteristics of the instruction actually offered are not
clearly detailed in reports of some of these investigations.  Among
the variants one finds (1) the extent to which certain heuristical
maxims are specifically taught, (2) the manner in which maxims are
isolated and illustrated as the single or primary tactic in a problem
solution, and (3) the degree to which an "heuristic" question or
advice is explicitly stated as opposed to being modeled but never
"pointed at."

     Notable results.  Both conclusion-oriented and exploratory inves-
tigations are represented in the studies reveiwed here.  Obviously
caution must be exercised in accepting the results.  Yet certain find-
ings appear to be evident across several of these studies:

     1.  A student's background knowledge of mathematics appears to
         be a dominant factor in successful mathematical problem-
         solving performances.  This observation supports the
         importance of carefully building-up the problem solver's
         knowledge of mathematical ideas.  However, the relation-
         ships among instructional variables and problem-solving
         competence are not clear from this research.  In particular,
         it is unclear what effects explicit "instruction in heuris-
         tical methods" may have upon knowledge structures,
         especially within problem-solving tasks.

     2.  Students given special treatments which feature problematic
         tasks and solving processes are often rated as relatively
         better or improved problem solvers.  Thus, solving problems
         and attending to solving methods do appear to obtain posi-
         tive results.  Yet, beyond this global maxim, "solve
         problems and reflect upon your solutions," considerably
         more detailed information is needed to guide mathematics
         teachers and students.  Some potential directions are
         suggested by the instructional and task variables included
         in these investigations.

     3.  Certain heuristical ploys or maxims appear from these
         studies to be more commonly taught or used.  These included
         trial and error, successive approximation, working back-
         wards, drawing a pattern or representation, and inductive
         pattern searching.  Perhaps these are more immediately used

-------
                                34
         because they are among the solving tactics used by
         humans in coping with all manner of task situations.
         They are deeply habituated and, therefore, more
         naturally and automatically called upon.  As Polya
         noted, the qualities of generality and common sense
         may be significant in obtaining heuristical competence.

     4.  The influence of idiosyncratic traits which a student
         brings to a problem-solving episode is unclear.  Few of
         these investigations attempted to study subject varia-
         bles.  Yet it seems crucial to effecting improved
         mathematical problem solving to be clear about the roles
         played by a student's aptitudes, preferences, cognitive
         structures, memory, learning styles, or personality.
         Furthermore, it may be important to discern individual
         trait (i.e., relatively stable and long-term) and state
         (or situational) factors in problem solving.
              'Looking Ahead":  Toward Coordinated Research of
                    Mathematical Problem Solving
     One of the unique features of a research consortium ought to be
the manifestation of a more concerted thrust on the problems under
consideration than the individual investigators, working separately,
might produce.  Put another way, the "whole should somehow become
greater than the simple sum of all parts."  Coordinated team research
in an area as complex as the learning and teaching of mathematical
problem solving will not be easy.  Yet perhaps the conditions for
creating focussed research efforts and results are now only becoming
existent in mathematical education in this country.

     This section examines certain points of view regarding prospec-
tive research in the Problem-Solving Project.  These ideas are
expressed to engender discussion both during and following the
Research Workshop.  It must be clear that all committed participants
in the intellectual consortia connoted by the GCSLTM must shape its
eventual contributions.  And this will certainly be a long-term
effort.  To execute change, real change, in the teaching and learn-
ing of mathematical problem solving in our schools will require no
less.

     The choice of emphasis in "instruction in heuristical methods"
will cast a certain direction to the Problem-Solving Project.  Inher-
ent in this choice are several hypotheses.  Heuristical methods of
problem solving:

     a.  can be learned,

     b.  can be taught,

-------
                                  35
     c.  if effectively used, do Improve problem-solving perfor-
         mances , and

     d.  along with its pedagogical counterpart, heuristical
         methods of teaching, can become a viable part of mathe-
         matical curricula.

In a global sense it could become the overriding mission of the Problem-
Solving Project to study and/or test these general hypotheses or conjec-
tures.  At the same time the focus on "instruction in heuristical
methods" must not inordinately constrain our research.  While having
a definite "applied" (i.e., classroom-oriented) characteristic, we must
also recognize and encourage more basic, theory-oriented efforts.  The
pattern of the recent Soviet research, incorporating both aspects, may
be a useful paradigm to follow.
Directions for Future Research

     Several general features of investigations to be encouraged in our
consortium are next proposed.  First, the processes, per se, of mathe-
matical problem solving must be studied.  Most past mathematics education
research which considered problem-solving outcomes has examined various
treatments and dependent measures that ignore the actual processes used
by the subjects during their problem-solving acts.  The solution (i.e.,
final "answer" or proof) of a mathematics problem, however lucidly set
down, is typically an inadequate trace of the processes used to arrive
at that solution object.  Ample direction for studying the cognitive
processes used by students in problem solving can be found in the works
of Brownell (1942), Duncker (1945), Buswell (1956), Wertheimer (1959),
Polya (1962),  Kilpatrick (1969), and Kantowski (1974).  Of particular
interest is the emphasis taken by a number of Soviet researchers in
studying the dynamics of mental activity during mathematical problem
solving (Kilpatrick and Wirszup, 1969),

     Secondly, the problems used with subjects should be non-trivial
mathematical problems of the sort they might meet in the classroom.
By a mathematical problem is meant a challenge encountered in a task
environment, which is itself perhaps only partially known to the sub-
ject, wherein the concepts, relations, operations, transformational
procedures, and models of mathematics provide the major elements or
vehicles for solving the challenge.  Laboratory studies of the psycho-
logist have rarely dealt directly with the complex behavior appropriate
for solving a challenging mathematics problem.  Many reasons can be
noted for selecting simple tasks, such as card sorting or level pull-
ing.  At the same time mathematics educators have generally hesitated
to apply any conclusions stemming from such research because the tasks
have been unrelated to the type of mathematical problems posed by the
mathematics teacher.  To assure relevance to mathematical education,
we should emphasize commonly used as well as nonroutine settings which
utilize appropriate mathematical concepts, principles, and skills
either known or readily learned by the subjects.

-------
                                  36
     The research methodology should make major use of qualitative
methods, small groups of subjects, and long-term genetic approaches
to study the learning and^ development of problem-solving competence^
There may be a growing sense of the need for the careful conduct of
clinical investigations in mathematics education research.  Brownell
noted promising changes in psychological research on problem solving,
including the setting of problems which "mean" something to the sub-
ject, concentrating attention on not merely the errors and successes
but on the way the subject proceeds to attack and solve the problem,
and attaching greater importance "to qualitative descriptions of sig-
nificant behavior to supplement or to replace purely quantitative
descriptions" (Brownell, 1942, p. 419),  Certainly the influence of
Piaget and his followers in demonstrating the efficacy of such method-
ologies has been great.  The recent appearance of the series, Soviet
Studies in the Psychology of Learning and Teaching Mathematics (Kil-
patrick and Wirszup, 1969) has generated further interest in approaches
aimed at penetrating into the child's thoughts to analyze his mental
processes.  Menchinskaya (1969) described various methodologies used
within the genetic approach, including the "teaching experiment" in
which study is combined with pedagogical influence and entire classes
of children are involved over a number of years in order that more
valid judgments about the changes that occur in mental activity as a
result of instruction might be made.  Kilpatrick advocates this
methodology by suggesting that the researcher "who chooses to investi-
gate problem solving in mathematics is probably best advised to under-
take clinical studies of individual subjects. . .because our ignorance
in this area demands clinical studies as precursors to larger efforts"
(Kilpatrick, 1969, p. 532).

     Wilson (1973) discussed the role, features, and credibility of
clinical intervention research.  Three major purposes research must
fulfill were identified:  generating hypotheses with antecedent prob-
abilities   confirming hypotheses, and constructing guiding models
and explanatory theories.  Theory construction was assigned to a
class referred to as Analytic-Synthetic Research while the confirming
of hypotheses was assigned to Experimental Research.  Normative
Research deals with activities designed to generate hypotheses con-
cerning facts and those connections between facts which exist in
nature.  Clinical Intervention Research, also conducted from the
generative purpose, is aimed at producing hypotheses about the connec-
tions among facts which might be brought into natural existence by
some intervention.  Among the distinguishing features of Clinical
Intervention Research, Wilson noted the methods of data collection
(emphasizing interviews), the type of data collected (primarily qual-
itative), the analyses performed on these data (often categorical),
the selection of subjects (typically non-random) the number of
subjects (usually only a small group), the length of time for subject
involvement (extended periods), the nature of the treatments (loosely
pre-planned but dynamically modified for individual subjects), and
the contents of reports (extensive, systematic descriptions of treat-
ments and apparent effects coupled with a conscious, guided search for
patterns among the idiosyncratic performances that can be translated
into testable hypotheses).

-------
                                  37
     A research methodology which seeks to elicit information about
the subjects' cognitive processes must utilize carefully conceptual-
ized task environments wherein the subject can operate naturally,
openly, and productively.  Our tasks must embody the potential for
stimulating cognition either directly or isomorphically characterized
as mathematical thought;  Within such task environments our subjects
would operate with other participants or, at times, with only the
experimenter.  The productive problem solving of children working
together toward a common goal will have direct importance for class-
room applications of this research.  Although we can recognize the
inadequacies and potential distortions, we should explore probing
techniques to encourage the subject to give a verbal self-report of
what he is or has been thinking, thereby prompting a phenomenological,
steam-of-consciousness record of mental processing.  Thus, to summar-
ize, in the current state of knowledge with respect to mathematical
problem-solving behavior, primary emphasis must now be given to
designing appropriate task environments, carefully observing over a
long period the individual child's spontaneous and learned problem-
solving behavior, reporting detailed case studies directed at portraying
the child's learning and development of problem-solving competence, and
generating hypotheses, procedures, measuring instruments, and designs
for future experimental investigations,

     The primary emphasis in our immediate research should be on con-
ducting "teaching experiments" (in the _Soviet_sense_) to obtain Jfurther
detailed information on mathematical problem-solving heuristics as
teachable-learnable-transferable knowledges.  These investigations
would likely have school-based, yet clinical, treatments which:  (a)
explicitly teach heuristical precepts within mathematical problem
solving, (b) use "heuristic teaching" methods, and  (c) study the
cognitive prerequisites for, and mental processes of, acquiring and
using such general precepts in mathematical problem solving.  Results
of such investigations should lead to empirically derived "maps" of
the domains to be considered in learning and teaching problem solving.
Careful delineations of taxonomies or typologies of problems, "heuris-
tics" or "strategies," and mathematical knowledge structures need  to
be formulated.

     At the same time, it seems of paramount importance to sponsor  and
encourage analytical research.  Little or no substantive theory-
building has been done relative to the teaching, learning, or using
of heuristical methods in mathematical problem solving.  In  recent
years there has been a notable increase in the interest of mathematics
education researchers for studying the use of heuristical advice in
problem solving.  Yet these scattered investigations have not been
predicated upon, nor led to, any but the narrowest of theoretical
bases.  The assumptions upon which an instructional emphasis in heur-
istical methods is based must be better explicated.  Those variables
that may be accounting for productive problem-solving performances,
learner difficulties in problem solving, and effective teaching and

-------
                                38
modeling of problem solving must also be identified and defined.   In
short, fundamental questions need to be systematically generated  and
studied.  Which heuristical maxims are "teachable objects"?  What
teaching "moves" or strategies might foster the acquisition and use
of heuristic?  What is the nature of a child's learning with respect
to heuristic?  To what extent might a learner's stage of cognitive
development account for the ease or difficulty in acquiring or using
heuristical methods during problem solving?  Are there useful taxono-
mies or typologies of mathematical problems for illustrating various
heuristical maxims?  These types of questions demand extensive analyses
as well as sensitive empirical treatment.

     The interface of available theoretical formulations in cognitive
deve^lggment psychology and in teaching strategies with our heuristical
emphases must be considered.  The proposed emphasis in "instruction in
heuristic methods" necessitates concerns for teaching factors.  The
model upon which the Teaching Strategies Project of the GCSLTM is based
does not include an analysis of "moves" in teaching problems, in  teach-
ing about problem solving, or more specifically in teaching heuristical
maxims (though the latter might well be construed to be principles or
generalizations within the present model).  Yet the power and general-
izability of this teaching strategies model suggests that its fundamental
features may be useful in building a similar model for teaching heuristi-
cal methods and problem solving.  Such model building and subsequent
model testing ought to be a central feature of our future work.  The
production of a potentially huge bank of recorded lessons or episodes
from our "teaching experiments" would allow an easy access to a variety
of excellent teaching exhibits to be analyzed in such model building.

     The relationships between cognitive developmental psychology and
the development of mathematical problem-solving competence must be
studied.  In particular, aspects of Piaget's concrete and formal oper-
ational thought should be examined for possible connection with the
learning and use of heuristical methods.  Conscious use of heuristics
in problem solving would seem to involve thought which encompasses
multiple operations, combinatorial processes, isolation of task varia-
bles, logical operations, flexibility (e.g., reversibility-reciprocity)
or other aspects of Piaget's theory.  We ought to be able to teach the
heuristical questions as items of knowledge.  But can we teach all sub-
jects to selectively use such heuristical advice?  Polya noted the
importance of the mental operations implied by an heuristic precept.
We need to map out in finer detail how mental operations and structures
may describe contingencies for successful problem solving.  That is, a
more penetrating analysis is needed of the cognitive operations essential
to allow the mind to consciously employ executive control over, and
choice among, the problem-solving strategies known to the student.
In particular, caution must be exercised that the operativity we wish
to see exhibited in the application of heuristical methods of problem
solving does not become mechanical rule-use by students who have been
unable to assimilate such knowledge.

-------
     Perhaps an equally viable question would pursue the apparent
effects of heuristical knowledge on cognitive operativity.  To be
sure, many of the task environments used by Piagetians are problem-
solving ventures.  A subject's spontaneity in approaching and dealing
with the task is often crucial to ascertaining the stage of opera-
tivity.  Yet heuristical knowledge would conceivably influence the
fashion in which a subject encoded and operated on a task.  For
example, would subjects who knew how to approach problems with quite
well-organized, inductive pattern searching strategies and who did
in fact solve some of the classical formal operations tasks (e.g.,
pendulum, balance beam, hidden magnet) as an apparent result, have
essentially been accelerated in their development?

-------
                                40
                              References
Ashton, Sister M. R.  Heuristic methods in problem solving in ninth
     grade algebra.  Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford Univer-
     sity, 1962.

Brownell, W. A.  Problem solving.  In The psychology of learning,
     Forty-first Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of
     Education, part II, 1942.

Buswell, G. T., & Kersh, B. Y.  Patterns of thinking in solving problems.
     University of California Publications in Education, 1956, 12, 63-148.

Butts, T.  Problem solving in mathematics.  Glenview, Illinois:  Scott,
     Foresman, 1973.

Covington, M. V. & Crutchfield, R. S.  Facilitation of creative problem
     solving.  Programmed Instruction, 1965, 4_(4) ,  3-5, 10.

Duncker, K.  On problem-solving.  Psychological Monographs, 1945, 58,
     (5, Whole No. 270).

Goldberg, D. J.  The effects of training in heuristic methods in the
     ability to write proofs in number theory.  Unpublished doctoral
     dissertation, Columbia University, 1973.

Higgins, J. L.  A new look at heuristic teaching.  The Mathematics
     Teacher, 1971, 64(6), 487-495.

Jennan, M.  Instruction in problem solving and an analysis oj[ structural
     variables that contribute to problem-solving difficulty (Tech. Rep.
     No. 180).  Stanford:  Institute for Mathematical Studies in the
     Social Sciences, Stanford University, 1971-

Jones, P. S. (Ed.).  A history of mathematics education in the United
     States and Canada.  Washington:  National Council of Teachers of
     Mathematics, 1970.

Kantowski, E. L.  Processes involved in mathematical problem solving.
     Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1974.

Kilpatrick, J.  Analyzing the solution of word problems in mathematics:
     An exploratory study.  Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford
     University, 1967.

Kilpatrick, J.  Problem solving and creative behavior in mathematics.
     In J. W. Wilson & L. R. Carry (Eds.)» Reviews  of recent research
     in mathematics education.  Studies in mathematics (Vol. 19).
     Stanford:  School Mathematics Study Group, 1969.

-------
                                41

Kilpatrick, J. , & Wirszup, I.  (Eds.)-  Soviet studies In the psychology
     of learning and teaching mathematics (6 vols.).  Stanford, California:
     School Mathematics Study Group, 1969-1972.

Larsen, C. M.  The he_uris_tic_ standpoint in the teaching of elementary
     calculus.  Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University,
     1960.

Lucas, J. F.  An exploratory study on the diagnostic Egjgchin_g_o_f heuristic
     problem-solving strategies in calculus.  Unpublished doctoral
     dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1972.

McClintock, C. E.  The effect of verbalization of he_uristics_ on transfer
     of learning.  Paper presented at the National Council of Teachers
     of Mathematics 53rd Annual Meeting, Denver, Colorado, April, 1975.

Menchinskaya, N. A.  Fifty years of Soviet instructional psychology.
     In J. Kilpatrick & I. Wirszup (Eds,), Soviet studies in the psychology
     of learning and teaching mathematics, (Vol. 1).  Stanford, Cali-
     fornia:  School Mathematics Study Group, 1969.

Olton, R., Wardrop, P., Covington, M., Goodwin, W., Crutchfield, R.,
     Klausmeir, H., & Ronda, T.  The development of productive thinking
     skills in fifth-grade children  (Tech. Rep. No. 34).  Madison,
     Wisconsin:  Wisconsin Research and Development Center for Cognitive
     Learning, 1967.

Polya, G.  How_ to solve it (2nd ed.).  Garden City, N. Y.:  Doubleday,
     1957.

Polya, G.  Mathematical discovery:  On understanding, learning, and
     teaching problem solving. (Vol. 1).  New York:  Wiley, 1962.

Polya, G.  Mathematical, discovery:  On understanding, learning, and
     teaching problem solving.  (Vol. 2).  New York:  Wiley, 1965.

Riedesel, C. A.  Problem solving:   Some suggestions from research.
     The Arithmetic Teacher, 1969, 16, 54-58.

Simon, H. A., & Newell, A.  Human problem solving:  The state of the
     theory in 1970.  American Psychologist, 1971, 26, 145-159.

Smith, J. P.  The effect of general versus specific heuristics in mathe-
     matical jjroblem-splving tasks.  Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
     Columbia University, 1973.

Suydam, M. N.  Annotated Compilations of Research on Secondary School
     Mathematics, 1930-1970.  Washington, B.C.:  U. S. Department of
     Health, Education and Welfare, Project No. l-C-004,1972.

Talyzina, N. F.  Properties of deductions in solving geometry problems.
     In J. Kilpatrick & I. Wirszup (Eds.), Soviet Studies in the psycho-
     logy of learning and teaching mathematics, (Vol. 4).  Stanford,
     California:  School Mathematics Study Group, 1970.

-------
                                42

Vos, K.  The effects of three instructional strategies on problem-
     solving behaviors in secondary school mathematics.  Journal for
     Research in Mathematics Education, 1976, 1(5),  264-275.

Webb, N.   An exploration of mathematical problem-solving processes.
     Paper presented at the meeting of the American  Educational Research
     Association, Washington, C.D., April, 1975.

Wertheimer, M.  Productive thinking. New York:  Harper, 1959.

Wickelgren, W. A.  How to solve problems:  Elements  of a theory of
     problems and pjroblem solving.  San Francisco:  W. H. Freeman
     and Company, 1974.

Wilson, J. W.  Generality of heuristics as an instructional variable.
     Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University,  1967.

Wilson, J.  An epistemologically based system for classifying research
     and the role of clinical intervention research  in that system.
     Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational
     Research Association, New Orleans, February, 1973.

-------
                                    43
                       The  Teaching Experiment and

                     Soviet Studies of Problem Solving

                           Mary  Grace  Kantowski

                         University of Florida


      The purpose  of  this paper  is  threefold;  first,  to  examine  the
 typically  Soviet  research  methodology known as the  teaching  experiment;
 next,  to review several of the  Soviet Studies related to mathematical
 problem solving in which some form of the  teaching  experiment was used;
 and,  finally, to  reflect on ways in which  aspects of this methodology
 could be applied  in  this country in research  dealing with the processes
 involved in mathematical problem solving.


                     Rationale for  a New Methodology


      A perspicacious grasp of the  Soviet concept  of  the "teaching experi-
 ment"  requires a  thorough  understanding of the forces that led  to its
 conception and some  reflection  on  the  rationale for  its development.

      Among the primary forces that  necessitated the  evolution of a new
 research methodology in the U.S.S.R. was the  influence  of the philosophy
 of the collective in the post-revolutionary   society.   The Soviet attitude
 toward learning and  instruction was a  strong  reaction to the concept of
 the class system of pre-revolutionary  Russia.  In the spirit of the
 theory of dialectical materialism  the  Soviets  assumed instruction, not
 native ability, to be the  major factor in intellectual  achievement.
 They believed that except  for cases involving organic damage or severe
 retardation, all children  have  the  same potential for academic accom-
 plishment. 1

     Whereas learning theories and pedagogical principles had previously
 been based on experimental  and theoretical psychology,  the Marxist per-
 cention of the dynamic interrelationship between  pedagogy and psychology
 emphasized the influence of instruction and content  on  psychological
     lOnly a minority of pedagogical researchers led by V. A. Krutetskii
are somewhat dissonant with this main stream of thought.  Krutetskii,
whose research will be discussed in a later section of this paper, is
investigating variation in ability.

-------
                                   44
growth.  This point of view compelled a search for a research methodology,
different from the "cross-sectional" type of investigation, that would
permit researchers to observe qualitative effects of various forms of
instruction.  Such a direction obviously mandated the organization of
research as well as techniques that would allow researchers not merely
to observe complex processes involved in learning such content as
reading, grammar, and mathematics, but that would, in fact, influence
the development of these processes.  The new research methods would have
to include longitudinal observation and evaluation; they would have to
permit a researcher to study changes in mental activity as well as the
effects of planned instruction on such activity.

     To this end, pedagogical and psychological research were tied
together in their organization under the Academy of Pedagogical Sciences
(Reitman, 1962).  Since the primary value of psychological research under
the Soviet regime was seen to be the improvement of instruction, Academy
studies of thinking necessarily involved the teaching methods most
effective in producing learning and independent thinking, and, conversely,
studies related to instruction dealt with complex mental processes.
                        The Influence of Vygotsky
     The "teaching experiment" grew out of the "individual psychological
experiment" introduced in the twenties by Lev Serayonovich Vygotsky, the
psychologist-educator who left an indelible mark on Soviet pedagogical
research before his early death in 1934.

     In the Marxist tradition, Vygotsky asserted that specifically human
mental processes are not inborn but formed, and that their development
is totally dependent on how they are taught.  He characterized intellec-
tual development as evolutionary or shaped by adaptation to external
environment and not embryonic which he interpreted as development flowing
more or less smoothly according to a stereotype.  According to Vygotsky,
all mental processes occur only by acquisition as a result of internali-
zations after some external activity (El'konin, 1967; Gal'perin, 1967).
Moreover, clinical data collected in early studies convinced him that
the development of certain mental processes was accompanied by changes
in cognitive structure at various levels of sophistication of function
of these processes (Kostyuk, 1968).  He found, for example, that the
processes of analysis, synthesis, comparison, and generalization exhibi-
ted definite levels, and noted positive effects of various pedagogical
practices on these levels.

     These factors in the revolutionary concept of the formation of men-
tal processes, the indication of changes in cognitive structure with
mental growth in certain operations and this conviction of the primacy
of instruction in mental development led to Vygotsky's conception of a
genetic instructional research methodology that would focus in the
qualitative aspects of thinking and learning.  He conceived of an
"instructional experiment" that would be a systematic reproduction of

-------
                                   45
processes as they develop under various instructional procedures.   In
his "laboratory," often the school setting, he tried to follow the
course of development by "experimentally evoking the genesis of volun-
tary attention" (El'konin, 1967).  Because the processes were observed
only periodically, Vygotsky attached great importance to his concept
of the "zone of proximal development" (Gal'perin, 1967; Kostyuk, 1968),
where the passage from lack of knowledge or lack of ability to operate
to possession of knowledge of operational ability and the corresponding
change in cognitive structure could be observed.

     Perhaps artificially, Vygotsky distinguished between "simple" and
"scientific" concepts, a difference not in the content of concepts
but in the way concepts are mastered (El'konin, 1967; Menchinskaya,
1969b; Talyzina, 1962).  He saw the former as learned spontaneously
and inefficiently from "object to definition" through daily experience
while the latter were learned from "definition to object" through
carefully planned instruction.  This distinction, which further empha-
sizes the Soviet view of the primacy of instruction was seen as
necessary to Vygotsky since he felt that in the acquisition of "scien-
tific" concepts and relationship between instruction and development
was most clear and capable of most complete investigation.  It was with
the development of "scientific" concepts that Vygotsky's studies
were concerned.^

     The method introduced by Vygotsky was, in a sense, modeling rather
than empirically studying the processes as they developed and, studying
the results of the learned behavior in a clinical setting.

     Vygotsky began using these genetic experimental techniques in
studying the relationship between language and thought.  His influence
soon spread to other disciplines, and although his basic concept of  the
pedagogical experiment remained the same, it began to  take on different
forms to correspond to varying research needs.


              Characteristics of the^ "Teaching Experiment"


     If one word had to be chosen to characterize the  "teaching experi-
ment," it would most likely be the term "dynamic" since it is movement
that interests the Soviet researchers—movement from ignorance to knowl-
edge, from one level of operation to another, from a problem to a
solution.  The aim of this research is to "catch" processes in their
development and to determine how instruction can optimally influence
these processes.  Unlike most experimental designs used in this country,
     2Recently, Menchinskaya  (1969b) and Talyzina  (1962) have  taken
issue with Vygotsky's assumption that concepts are best learned
"scientifically."  Studies  related to activity learning (Zykova, 1969;
Kalmykova, 1962) support their objections.

-------
                                    46
 the "teaching experiment"  cannot  be  completely  characterized by
 describing sampling  procedures, experimental  groups  and  test statistics.
 The label  is  actually  a  generic term for  a variety of  pedagogical
 research forms in which  the  strictly statistical  analysis of quantita-
 tive data  is  of less concern than the daily subjective analysis  of
 qualitative data.  Most  studies deal with some  aspect  of the formal
 school  situation although  the data are often  gathered  only  from  a
 sampling of "strong" "average" or "weak"  students who  are generally
 categorized and selected with the aid of  the  classroom teacher.  The
 data collected are often qualitative, obtained  in a  clinical setting
 by  recording  verbal  protocols for future  analysis.   Underlying this
 procedure  is  one of  the  salient features  of the "teaching experiment,"
 its compensatory nature.   The quantity of macroscopic  data  (such as
 objective  test  scores) generally  acquired in  an experimental study is
 exchanged  for microscopic  detail  of  processes observed using a small
 sample.  Probing interviews  and exchanges with  individual students add
 to  any  group  data collected  to support generalizations resulting in
 decisions  for future instructional sequences.

      Other  general characteristics of the "teaching  experiment"  include
 its longitudinal nature  (instructional treatment is  applied and data
 are gathered  over an extended period), the planning  of instruction in
 the light of  observations  made during the previous session, and extensive
 co-operation  among classroom teachers and researchers.   It is proced-
 urally  acceptable to give  hints to the subjects during testing, so that
 any learning  in  the  testing  situation may also  be observed.  In most
 cases results are reported in the  form of a narrative  that includes an
 analysis of observed behaviors and conclusions  drawn from the analysis.
 Any quantitative data are  generally  reported using descriptive statis-
 tics.   Inferential test  statistics are seldom used.
                       Some "Teaching Experiments"
     Menchinskaya broadly defines the "teaching experiment" as "study
combined with pedagogical influence" (1969a).  She describes two
forms of this research widely in use (Menchinskaya, 1969b).  The first
is the "experiencing" form in which only one mode of instruction is
employed and observations are made in a clinical setting to determine
its influence on mental processes.  No explicit comparison is made to
any other instructional procedure.

     The Gal'perin and Georgiev study (1969) and the follow up study
by El'konin (1961) are examples of research using the "experiencing"
form.  Both involved the introduction of mathematical concepts using
the unit of measurement instead of the concept of number.  In the
El'konin study, although no explicit comparisions are made, the sub-
jects chosen were judged to be very low in mathematical concepts
initially.   Thus, the fact that this group learned all mathematical
concepts and skills required at their grade level implied a judgment
regarding the value of the method.  The study employed the latest form
in the evolution of the "teaching experiment" and one that should be of

-------
                                   47
interest to American researchers who are attempting to bridge the gap
between research and what actually occurs in the classroom.   The experi-
mental classroom used was equipped with one-way glass and with a TV
camera lens mounted in the classroom and connected to a screen in the
adjoining laboratory, which was also equipped with booths for individual
experiments.  All group and individual sessions were recorded on tape
for future analysis.  Although the general course outline and content
to be covered were determined in advance, the experimenters, teachers
and aides observed the class sessions held during the day, discussed
the lessons and planned activities and instruction for the following
session on the basis of what occurred each day.  The individual experi-
ments highlight another feature of the "teaching experiment," that of
probing for hunches on which to base new instructional strategies.
Again, the dynamic nature of the "teaching experiment" is evident here
as the experimenters attempt to capture processes as they are being
formed and to determine optimal strategies of instruction.  This proce-
dure may be useful as a preliminary to pilot testing to determine
plausible hypothesis to be tested in future  experimental studies.

     The "experiencing" mode of the teaching experiment was also used
by Krutetskii in his studies of mathematical abilities (1965, 1969,
1973).  The investigations were conducted by Krutetskii and his stu-
dents between the years 1959 and 1965 using students from the second
through the tenth grades.  By analyzing solutions of carefully organized
sets of mathematical problems generated over periods of about two years
with the same students, Krutetskii was able to delineate characteristics
of studants with high ability in mathematics.  The organization of the
problem presentation was instructional; as the problems were solved,
mental processes were observed in their development.  The problems
included those requiring generalizations and algebraic proofs, and those
with visual-graphic and oral-logical components,  among others.  Krutetskii
emphasized that although some quantitative data were gathered (e.g., the
number of problems solved and the time to solution), the dynamic indices,
such as progress in qualitative aspects of problem solving, were more
valuable than the static, quantitative ones.  These are reflected in  the
components Krutetskii enumerated in the structure of mathematical abilities,
namely (1) the formalized perception of mathematical material (2) quick
and sweeping generalization of mathematical material (3) curtailment of
thought (4) flexibility of thought  (5) striving for economy and  (6)  a
mathematical memory. -*  In a recent publication, Krutetiskii took a
definite stand on the existence of  levels of ability  (1973), a position
opposed to the basic Soviet philosophy.
     3 In his earlier writings Krutetskii  included  spatial skills  in  the
 structure, but later removed it from the  "obligatory"  structure.

-------
     The second form of the "teaching experiment" is the "testing" mode,
one more closely related to our own experimental studies.   This pro-
cedure was used by Kalmykova (1962) in a study related to mathematical
applications in physics.  Aspects of the research that clearly distin-
guishes it from our experimental studies include the type of data
collected and the form of analysis of these data.  With the help of the
classroom teacher the subjects in the study were divided into "weak"
and "strong."  One half of each group was assigned to each of two methods
of instruction—method "A" which was essentially expository and in which
the teacher outlined the procedures to follow in completing the exercises,
and method "B" which was essentially a heuristic teaching technique.  A
variable called "rate of learning" was determined by the number of problems
needed in the instruction and the time necessary for a subject to com-
plete exercises independently.  Kalmykova states that there was no
significant difference in the effects of two methods for the "strong"
students.  In spite of the fact that the average number of problems and
the time required for mastery were not significantly different even in
the case of the "weak" pupils she concludes, one the basis of analysis
of clinically obtained data, that method "B" was superior for the "weak"
students since they exhibited higher levels of analytic-synthetic activity
in the solution of the control problems.

     In another study Kalmykova (1975) uses a form that does not clearly
fall into either the "experiencing" or the "testing" category but con-
tains elements of both.  In her initial research study on analysis and
synthesis in problem solving, Kalmykova observed various teachers as they
taught problem solving in elementary school classrooms and then examined
the problem solving behaviors of their students.  She analyzed in detail
the instructional strategies of one particularly successful teacher,
V.D. Petrova, and suggested elements that should be included in all
problem solving instruction based on her analysis of Petrova's techniques.
Kalmykova herself then applied these techniques with some success in
instructing a group of "weak" students.  Petrova's method of instruction,
which emphasized both analysis and synthesis in problem solving, was
compared to the more structured "classical analysis" methods used by the
other teachers.  These comparisons used observations of problem solving
behaviors of the students from all participating classes.  Thus, both
the "experiencing" form and the "testing" form were used to some extent
by Kalmykova in the same study.


               The "Teaching Experiment" and Our Research
     Any attempt to compare the Soviet methodology to research designs
used in this country would be analagous to an attempt to answer the
question:  "Who is the better athlete Chris Evert or Olga Korbut?"
Just as an athlete is judged on standards related to her event, each
research methodology must be examined in the light of its purposes
and the philosophy of education encompassing it.  The Soviets are
concerned with the qualitative aspects of mathematics learning and

-------
                                   49
problem solving.   Thus far we in this country, with few exceptions,
have focused on the quantitative.  Perhaps the answer to effective
problem solving research lies in a compromise—a merger of the two
methodologies through studies that involve both aspects.

     Paradoxically, the essential differences between the "teaching
experiment" and research in this country account for what could here
be considered the most severe limitations as well as the desirable
strengths of the  Soviet methodology.  Because the instruction is often
determined by what is found in preliminary analysis and the duration
of the experiments is often a year or longer, this approach introduces
variables that would be considered invalidating to most American
researchers.  The time between the initial testing and the intro-
duction of the instruction could result in learning on the part of
subjects who were identified as not having the desired skills initially.
Because analysis  is logical, the introduction of hints during testing is
not considered undesirable in Soviet research.  Experimental control in
our sense gives way to the opportunity to "catch" the learning of a
concept or strategy and to suggest instructional techniques to insure
mastery.  On the  other hand, the instructional experiment allows the
researcher to observe how a subject is operating and to determine levels
of sophistication (for example, elegance in problem solving) instead of
mere numbers of correct solutions.  Such diagnostic techniques permit
the discovery of erroneous concepts as well as "strokes of genious."

     Few researchers in mathematics education in this country have con-
cerned themselves with the detailed study of the development of pro-
cesses in mathematics.  Kantowski (1974) investigated processes used in
the development of skills in solving geometry problems; Lucas (1972)
and Goldberg (1973) studied processes related to problem solving in the
Calculus and number theory, respectively.

     Although process research in this country has been sparse, several
recent studies related to problem solving could be modified to include
a process component.  For example, in the Wilson (1967) study involving
the level of generality of heuristics used in instruction, a matrix
sampling technique could be used and qualitative data collected from
representative subjects in each of the cells.  Analysis of such data
could serve as a valuable supplement to the statistical analysis of the
quantitative data by providing information on how the development of
processes is affected by the level of generality of the heuristics used
in instruction in each of the content areas, and perhaps by suggesting
other instructional techniques.

     Another possibility would be to randomly select individuals from
intact classes such as those used in the Goldberg (1973) and Lucas (1972)
studies and to follow identical instructional techniques with these
individuals while gathering qualitative data from their verbal and
written protocols along with the quantitative data from the remainder
of the classes.  Such observations made using subjects on various ability
levels could suggest Aptitude-Treatment-Interaction studies for teaching
problem solving.   Other suggestions for studies related to process
research may be found in Kantowski  (1974).

-------
                                   50
     The gathering of qualitative data is but one step in progress toward
understanding the processes of mathematical thinking.  Methods of
analyzing the data are needed, and more importantly, methods for com-
municating the results to other researchers, and to classroom teachers
must be explored.  Finally, the ultimate goal is to find ways to use the
data to improve classroom instruction and to positively affect mathe-
matics learning and problem solving in the classroom.

-------
                                   51
                               References
El'konin, D. B.   A psychological study in an experimental class.  Soviet^
     Education,  1961, 3_ (7), 3-10.

El'konin, D. B.   The problem of instruction and development in the works
     of L. S. Vygotsky.  Soviet Psychology, 1967, 5_ (3), 34-41.

Gal'perin, P. Ya.  On the notion of internalization.  Soviet^ Psychology,
     1967, 5_ (3), 28-33.

Gal'perin, P. Ya., and Georgiev, L. S.  The formation of elementary
     mathematical notions.  In J. Kilpatrick & I. Wirszup (Eds.),
     Soviet studies in the psychology^ of learning and teaching
     mathematics (Vol. 1).  Stanford, California:  School
     Mathematics Study Group, 1969.

Goldberg, D. J.   The effects of training in heuristic methods in the
     ability to write proofs in number theory.  Unpublished doctoral
     dissertation, Teacher's College, Columbia University, 1973.

Kalmykova, Z. I.  Dependence of knowledge assimilation  level on pupils'
     activity in learning.  Soviet^ EducatjLpn, 1958, 1^ (11), 63-68.

Kalmykova, Z. I.  Analysis  and synthesis as problem-solving methods.   In
     J. Kilpatrick, E. G. Begle,  I. Wirszup, & J. A. Wilson, (Eds.),
     Soviet studies^ in the  psychology of learning and teaching  mathe-
     matics  (Vol.11).  Stanford,  California:  School  Mathematics  Study
     Group, 1975.

Kantowski, M. G.  Processes involved in mathematical problem solving,
     Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University  of Georgia,  1974.

Kostyuk,  G.  S.  The problem of child development  in Soviet psychology.
     Soviet Psychology, 1968, 6  (3-4), 91-111.

Krutetskii, V. A.   Age peculiarities  in the development in  abilities
     in  students.   Soviet Education, 1965, 8^  (5),  15-27.

Krutetskii, V. A.   An investigation  of mathematical abilities  in school
     children.   In  J.  Kilpatrick & I. Wirszup  (Eds.), Soviet  studies  in
     the psychology of learning and  teaching mathematics^ (Vol.  2).
     Stanford, California:   School Mathematics  Study Group,  1969b.

Krutetskii,  V. A.   The problem of the formation and development of
     abilities.   Soviet  Education, 1973,  16,  127-145.

Lucas,  J. F.  An exploratory study in the diagnostic teaching of elemen-
     tary calculus.   Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
     Wisconsin,  1972.

-------
                                   52
Menchinskaya, N. A.  Fifty years of Soviet instructional psychology.  In
     J. Kilpatrick & I. Wirszup (Eds.), Soviet studies^ in the psychology
     of JLearning and teaching mathematics (Vol. 1).  Stanford, California:
     School Mathematics Study Group, 1969a.

Menchiaskaya, N. A.  The psychology of mastering concepts; fundamental
     problems and methods of research.  In J. Kilpatrick & I. Wirszup
     (Eds.), Soviet studies in the psychology of learning and teaching
     mathematics (Vol. 1).  Stanford, California:  School Mathematics
     Study Group, 1969b.

Reitman, W.  Some Soviet investigations of thinking, problem solving, and
     related areas.  In R. A. Bauer (Ed.), Some views on Soviet psychology.
     Washington:  American Psychological Association, 1962, 29-61.

Talyzina, N. F.  Manner of acquisition of initial scientific concepts.
     Soviet Psychology and Psychiatry, 1962, 1 (1), 39-42.

Vygotsky, L. S.  Thought and language.  Cambridge, Mass.:  MIT press, 1962.

Wilson, J. W.  Generality of heuristics as an instructional variable.
     Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1967.

Zykova, V. I.  Operating with concepts when solving geometry problems.
     In J. Kilpatrick & I. Wirszup (Eds.), Soviet studies in learning
     and teaching mathematics (Vol. 1).  Stanford, California:  School
     Mathematics Study Group, 1969.

-------
                                   53
         Mathematical Problem Solving In The Elementary School:

          Some Educational And Psychological Considerations*

                          Frank K.  Lester, Jr.

                           Indiana University


                              Introduction
     One of the most important goals of elementary school mathematics is
to develop in each child an ability to solve problems.  In recent years
more and more emphasis has been placed on problem solving in the
elementary mathematics curriculum.   A cursory look at the scope and
sequence charts of the most popular textbook series points out this
trend.  In each of these series problem solving is identified as one
of the key strands around which the mathematics program is built.  At
the same time there is concern among teachers, mathematicians, and
mathematics educators that these programs are doing a poor job of
developing problem solving ability in children.  Points of view which
are representative of the dissatisfaction with current programs are
found in the reports of the Snowmass Conference on the K-12 Curriculum
and the Orono Conference on the National Middle School Mathematics
Curriculum held during the summer of 1973.  These reports called for
extensive modification of current mathematics programs to include a
more systematic approach to providing instruction in problem solving.

     The current concern should raise a number of questions in the mind
of anyone interested in the mathematics education of children.  Exactly
what is problem solving?  Can students really be taught to be better
solvers?  If problem solving is so important and good problem solvers
are not being developed, what steps should be taken to change present
instructional practices?  Certainly an answer to the first question must
be obtained.  So, before proceeding any further a definition of problem
solving should be provided.
     *The author is indebted to Dr. Norman L. Webb and other members of
the Mathematical Problem Solving Staff at Indiana University for their
valuable suggestions.  The views expressed in this paper do not constitute
an official statement of policy regarding the goals of the Mathematical
Problem Solving Project.  Th author accepts sole responsibility for all
of the positions and views stated in this paper.

-------
                                    54
Definition  of  a  Problem

      A problem is a situation in which an individual  or  group is called
upon  to perform  a task for which there is no readily  accessible algorithm
which determines completely the method of solution.

      Any one of  a number of other definitions of  a problem would be
satisfactory for the purposes of this paper (e.g., Bourne, Ekstrand &
Dominowski, 1971; Davic, 1966; Henderson & Pingry, 1953; Newell & Simon,
1972).  Let it suffice to say that any reference  to a problem or problem
solving refers to a situation in which previous experiences, knowledge,
and intuition must be coordinated in an effort to determine an outcome
of that situation for which a procedure for determining  the outcome is
not known.   Thus, finding the length of the hypothenuse  of a right triangle
given the lengths of the two legs probably does not involve problem
solving for the  student who understands the Pythagorean Theorem, but
may be problem solving of a complex nature for the student who has not
been exposed to  the Pythagorean Theorem.

      Since  problem solving is viewed as such an important part of learning
mathematics, it  seems natural to analyze  carefully what  is involved in
the process so that effective instructional techniques can be developed.
There is little  or no argument on this point.  Everyone  agrees that serious
attention must be given to instructional  issues related to problem
solving.  However, beyond this point there is  little, if any, unanimity
of opinion concerning the process of problem solving.

      Even the most successful problem solvers  have difficulty in identify-
ing why they are successful, and even the best mathematics teachers are
hard  pressed to  pinpoint what it is that  causes their students to become
good  problem solvers.  Unfortunately, in  spite of the volumes that have
been  devoted to  problem solving what is now universally accepted know-
ledge about problem solving can be boiled down to George Polya's words
of advice to mathematics students:  "Use  your  head."  (Professor Polya's
final statement  in a presentation at the  1974  annual meeting of the
American Mathematical Society.)

      Out of frustration over an inability  to deal successfully with the
problem solving  dilemma,  mathematics  educators have turned to psychology
for guidance.  The nature of problem solving and the measurement of problem
solving ability  have  been the objects  of considerable attention by psycho-
logists  (representative reviews  of psychological research in problem solving
have been written by  Bourne  £. Dominowski, 1972; Davis, 1966;  Green,  1966).
Typically,  psychological  reports  of problem solving research begin with
a statement like:  "Research in  human  problem solving has a well-earned
reputation  for being  the  most chaotic  of all identifiable categories of
human  learning"  (Davis, 1966,  p.  36).   Indeed, it has only been during
the last twenty-five  years that  a  major point of view or technique has

-------
                                   55
developed which attempts to isolate the important variables which influ-
ence problem solving behavior.1

     There appear to be a number of reasons for this condition.  First,
a variety of tasks has been used in problem solving research.   The tasks
found in the literature include such diverse problems as matchstick,
Tower of Hanoi, jigsaw puzzles, anagram problems, concept identification
problems, arithmetic computation problems, and standard mathematics text-
book word problems.  Also, problem solving research has been conducted
by experimenters with quite different positions on the nature of problem
solving.  The traditional cognitive-Gestalt approach of such psychologists
as Wertheimer (1959), Maier (1970), and Duncker (1945) is quite different
from the associative learning theory approach characterized by the work
of Maltzman (1955) and the Kendlers (Kendler & Kendler, 1962).  More
recently, especially within the past fifteen years, considerable effort
has been devoted to the development of an information processing approach
to the study of problem solving.  The well-known work of Newell and Simon
(1972) is representative of the information processing view of the pro-
blem solving process.  Thus, although much exciting and potentially
fruitful work is being conducted by psychologists, very few definitive
answers to the questions concerning the nature of learning and instruction
in mathematical problem solving are available at the present time.  It
is likely that these answers will result only from several years of
intensive study that reflects a cooperative effort by mathematics
educators, psychologists, and classroom teachers.
Overview of This Paper

     The intent of this paper is to describe the philosophy and activities
of the Mathematical Problem Solving Project (MPSP) at Indiana University.
The paper will contain four main sections:

     1.  the critical issues and questions related to
         mathematical problem solving,

     2.  nature of the MPSP,

     3.  thrust of the work of MPSP at Indiana University, and

     4.  plans for future research.

     The main focus of this paper is on the research and development efforts
underway at Indiana University.  Included in this effort is a serious
     •"•Kilpatrick (1969) suggests that serious attention to problem solving
by mathematics educators has developed primarily within the last ten or
so years.

-------
                                     56
attempt  to  develop a conceptual framework for mathematical problem
solving.  The  development of such a framework will center on  the  creation
of a model  for mathematical problem solving.   Since the creation  of  such
a model  is  considered to be of utmost importance in developing a  frame-
work for future research and development efforts,  an extensive discussion
of models of problem solving is included.  It is hoped that the positions
posed  and the  efforts described will stimulate valuable discussion con-
cerning  the key issues related to mathematical problem solving in the
elementary  schools.
                   Critical Issues and Questions  Related

                      to Mathematical Problem Solving
      The opening sentence of this paper stated  that the development of
 children's  problem solving abilities  is a major goal of elementary school
 mathematics.   It is interesting that  while  few  educators would disagree
 with this claim there is little evidence that a serious attempt is being
 made to attain this goal.  No single  factor can be identified as causing
 this state  of affairs to exist.  Instead the problem can be attributed
 to a number of causes.   The following are among the most prominent:

      1.   Problem solving is the most  complex of  all intellectual
          activities;  consequently,  it is the most difficult intel-
          lectual ability to develop.

      2.   Elementary school mathematics textbooks typically are
          deleterious rather than facilitative in  developing
          problem solving skills and processes in children.

      3.   Elementary school teachers do not  view problem solving
          as a key feature of their  mathematics programs.

 Before  suggestions are  presented for  remedying the present situation it
 is  appropriate to elaborate on causes 2 and 3.

      It  is  the author's opinion that  the overwhelming-majority of the
 activities  presented  in elementary  mathematics texts as problems are
 actually little more  than exercises designed for practicing the use of
 a  formula of  algorithm.   A second criticism is that textbooks do not
 include  enough situations which involve real-world2 applications of
 mathematics.
      The term "real-world"  is  difficult to define since a real-world or
real-life problem for one person may not be a real-life problem for
another.  Although interest  rate and grocery shopping problems are very
real in the sense that such  problems are encountered daily by adults, they
are often not even problems  for children because children are not inter-
ested in them.

-------
                                   57
     The third cause is the result of several factors.  It is a fact
that most elementary school teachers perceive mathematics to be a static
and closed field of study.  To them mathematics is more mechanics than
ideas, and involves very little independent or original thought.  Of
course elementary teachers cannot be blamed for their perception of
mathematics since it is based primarily on educational experiences which
stressed memorization of rules, formulas, and facts.  However, the view
of mathematics which is held by elementary teachers is a part of a
vicious cycle which has developed.  Children are not learning to become
good problem solvers because their mathematics textbooks do not provide
appropriate opportunities for them to solve problems and because their
teachers do not view problem solving as important.  At the same time,
teachers do not view problem solving as important because it was not
given priority status when they studied mathematics.  This condition
cannot be rectified by attempting to convince preservice teachers of the
importance of problem solving.  At Indiana University preservice elemen-
tary school teachers are required to take nine semester hours of mathe-
matics and three semester hours of methods of teaching mathematics.
Even this uncommonly good situation does not allow sufficient time to over-
come ten or more years of "bad" experiences with mathematics.  Also,
young teachers are prone to model their teaching behavior after the
behavior of their supervising teachers.  Consequently, if little or no
provision is made for developing children's problem solving skills by a
student teacher's supervising teacher, it is unlikely that the student
teacher will consider problem solving as an important part of the mathe-
matics program.

     Remedies for the existing conditions cannot ignore the need to
improve current teacher training programs, but improved teacher training
is only a small part of the solution.  Even if teachers can be trained
to view mathematics as an area accessible through experimentation and
independent thought, they will probably resort to using whatever written
materials are available in the classroom and these materials are, for
the most part, not conducive to enhancing the development of problem
solving abilities.  Thus, serious and extensive efforts must begin to
develop exemplary instructional materials in mathematics which have
problem solving as their main focus.  The Mathematical Problem Solving
Project (MPSP), which will be described later, is attempting to satisfy
the need for such problem solving materials by producing a series of
modules devoted to the development of certain problem solving techniques
and by collecting and categorizing problems suitable for use in the
intermediate grades.

     Attempts to develop instructional materials of any type must involve
considerable reflection about the most important aspects of the topic
being considered.  In the course of developing modules which will teach
children fundamental skills and processes of problem solving the following
questions are among those which should be considered.

-------
                                   58
     1.  What kind and how much direction should be given in a
         module?

     2.  What instructional  format is best suited to teaching
         children how to solve problems?

     Of course, these are important questions, but they are not specifi-
cally related to mathematical problem solving.  Instead, they are ques-
tions which are raised by writers of any sort in instructional materials.
It is premature to attempt to answer these questions until answers to
several more basic questions are found.  Unfortunately, the knowledge that
exists about how children solve problems and how problem solving should
be taught is very limited.   For example, no confident answers have been
found for the most basic questions such as:

     1.  What prerequisite skills,abilities, etc. must children
         have to solve particular kinds of problems?

     2.  What aspects of the problem solving process can be
         taught to intermediate grade children?

         a.  Can children use various problem solving strategies
             effectively?

         b.  Can children learn to coordinate the cognitive processes
             which are needed in solving complex problems?

     Clearly the answers to  these questions to a certain extent must be
based upon the intuition and experience of the persons involved in
writing the materials.  However, it is equally as important that these
questions be attacked by considering the theoretical and research base
underlying the various views toward teaching problem solving.  It would
be most unfortunate to have  another curriculum project which devotes
all its energies to the development of materials to the exclusion of
attempting to further the scientific knowledge regarding learning and
instruction in mathematical  problem solving.-'

     The issues raised thus  far have been concerned primarily with the
role of problem solving in the existing mathematics curriculum and the
development of instructional materials.  Before these issues can be dealt
with in an appropriate way it is essential that several more fundamental
issues and questions be considered.  These issues include the four pre-
viously mentioned and are listed with some discussion following.

     1.  Can problem solving be taught?
     -*This view is also held by Richard Shumway and is presented in a
position paper prepared by him for the MPSP  (1974).

-------
                                   59
     2.   If problem solving can be "taught,"^ what type of experiences
         most enhance the development of this ability?

     3.   What are the specific characteristics of successful problem
         solvers?

     4.   What prerequisite skills, abilities, etc. and  what level of
         cognitive development must a student have in order to solve
         a particular class of problems?

     5.   Educators and psychologists generally agree that there are
         several factors which influence problem difficulty.  What are
         the primary determinants of mathematical problem difficulty
         for children in grades 4-6?

     6.   There are several motivation factors which influence children's
         ability and willingness to solve mathematical  problems.  For
         example:

         a.  What types of problems are interesting to  children in
             grades 4-6?

         b.  To what extent does a child's cognitive and emotional
             style influence her/his willingness to solve problems?

     7.   What problem solving strategies can children (grades 4-6) learn
         to use effectively?  More fundamentally, can problem solving
         strategies be taught which are generalizable to a class of
         problems?

     8.   Since problem solving is also important in nonmathematical
         areas, the question arises concerning the extent to which
         learning to solve various types of mathematical problems
         transfers^ to solving nonmathematical problems  (the issue is
         just as important if modified to read ". . . transfers to
         solving other types of mathematical problems").

     9.   There are a number of issues related to the method of instruction.
         Among the most important are:

         a.  Is the small group mode of instruction a better mode than
             either the large group mode or individual  instruction in
             terms of teaching problem solving?
     ^"Taught" is being used here in the sense that teaching can be
viewed as facilitating the understanding of or knowledge about something.
It does not imply necessarily direct intervention in the student's learn-
ing process.

-------
                                   60
         b.   What aspects of the problem solving process  should
             influence the choice of method of instruction?  For
             example, should the type of problem solving  strategy
             appropriate for a problem affect the instructional
             mode used?

         c.   The specific role of the teacher in problem  solving
             instruction is an open issue.   Are there certain  aspects
             of the problem solving process which suggest a more
             directive role by the teacher  than others?

         d.   How should problem solving instruction be organized  and
             sequenced?  For example, should specific skills (e.g.,
             making tables) be developed before attention is directed
             toward teaching a particular strategy? To what extent
             should a hierarchy (in the sense of Gagne 1970) be
             followed in planning instruction in problem  solving?


    10.  How do such characteristics of problems as difficulty,  interest,
         setting, strategy, and mathematical content relate to one
         another?

    11.  Several models of the problem solving process have been  suggested.
         Do any of these models adequately  describe mathematical  problem
         solving?  Is there a need for developing a model for  instruction
         in problem solving?  An instructional model might be  fundamentally
         different from a model of the solution process.
Specific Questions Under Study by the MPSP

     The MPSP at Indiana University has selected several of these issues
and questions for study:  namely, NOS. 1, 5,  6(a),  7,  and 11.   Since
these questions and issues have been given some careful thought, it is
appropriate to discuss them briefly.
     Question 1.  Can problem^ solving be taught?  Clearly,  this is the
most important question of all.  Kilpatrick's (1969)  review of mathe-
matical problem solving indicated that very little research has been
done regarding the influence of instruction on problem solving ability.
The answer to this question probably will not be determined until more is
known about the nature of solving problems and the relationships among
the many factors which influence mathematical problem solving.
     Question 5.  What are the primary determinants of mathematical
problem difficulty for children in grades 4-6?  Psychologists generally
focus on four main areas for investigating problem difficulty:  (a)
type of problem task; (b) method of presentation of the problem; (c)
familiarity of the problem solver with acceptable solution procedures
(strategies, skills, etc.); (d) problem size (e.g., a problem with several

-------
                                   61
dimensions, both relevant or irrelevant, is more difficult than a problem
having fewer dimensions).  Each of these areas has direct relevance for
elementary school mathematical problem solving.  Clearly, not all types
of problems are appropriate for children of this age.  What is less clear
is the best method of presenting particular classes of problems to chil-
dren.  Language factors, complexity of the problem statement, role of
concrete and visual materials, child's prior experiences, and type of
problem are among the several factors determining the most appropriate
method of presentation.  Much valuable information could be gained by
posing problems to students in different forms and versions and under
varying conditions.

     That the student's familiarity with acceptable solution procedures
is an important determinant of problem difficulty raises a number of
questions which must be considered.

     1.  Which skills and strategies are most important for aiding
         problem solving in mathematics in grades 4—6?

     2.  Which skills and strategies should be taught first?

     3.  Which, if any, strategies do students use naturally?

     4.  Which skills and strategies can be taught efficiently and
         effectively?  Can any be taught?

     5.  Should the skills (e.g., making a table) be developed before
         concentrating on teaching a strategy (e.g., pattern finding),
         or should they be developed as the strategy is taught?

     6.  Does teaching a particular strategy really improve problem
         solving ability in the sense that for any problem a student
         will be able to choose the most appropriate strategy to use?

     More questions are being raised than answers in this paper.  This
reflects the author's earlier statement that there are few definitive
answers to the questions about learning and instruction in mathematical
problem solving.  The questions posed in the preceding paragraph are no
exceptions.  However, despite the lack of answers based on firm research
evidence, there is considerable agreement that strategies can and should
be taught.  This claim will be discussed when Question 7 is considered.

     Issues related to problem size and problem complexity are a major
focus of the research efforts of the MPSP.  Since these efforts will be
discussed in the last section of this paper no more will be said about
problem size in this section.

     The four determinants of problem difficulty that have been discussed
are certainly not the only ones.  Rather, they are the ones to which
psychologists have devoted the most attention.  Maier (1970) stated that

-------
                                   62
there are several other important factors which make a problem difficult.
In determining a list of causes of difficulty, he began with the assump-
tion that there is no lack of knowledge on the student's part.  Based upon
this assumption he listed five potential causes of difficulty in addition
to the four that have already been mentioned:  (a) misleading incorrect
solutions, (b) type of demands made upon idea-getting processes versus
idea-evaluation processes, (c) difficulty in locating subgoals that can
be reached, (d) lack of motivation, and (e) high degree of stress.

     The factors which have been listed in the previous paragraphs
illustrate the extreme complexity of problem solving.  In addition
psychologists have determined these factors primarily through highly
controlled experimentation.  In many of the "laboratory" studies there
was no need to consider factors such as mathematical content, level
of understanding of concepts, processes, and skills, and environmental
influences since ability to perform the tasks used is not contingent upon
these factors.  Unfortunately, these factors are present in normal class-
room instruction.  Consequently, in addition to the determinants of pro-
problem difficulty which have already been mentioned, the teacher is
confronted with the task of dealing with even more confounding factors in
planning appropriate mathematical problem solving activities.
     Question 6 (a).  What types of problems are interesting to children
in grades 4-6?  This question cannot be answered without considerable
knowledge of a student's background, experiences, cognitive ability,
and psychological makeup.  There is substantial evidence that learning
is-enhanced when instruction is meaningful and relevant  to the student.
It is reasonable to expect that this is also the case in learning to
solve problems.  There are no hard-and-fast rules for determining if
a particular problem is interesting, but there are some  general rules-
of-thumb which can guide problem selection.

     1.  Be sure the problem statement (if written)  is easy for the
         student to read.

     2.  Use personal words and terms in the statement of the problem.
         Try to make the student feel like he is a part  of the problem.

     3.  Although "real-world" problems are often difficult to find,
         such problems have a high motivational value.  (Most of the
         "interesting" real-world problems are too sophisticated for
         the level of mathematical understanding which intermediate
         grade children have).

     4.  Encourage students to make up their own problems.

     5.   Do  not place the student in a stressful situation.   For example,
         insistence on getting a correct answer in a short period of
         time is a good way to kill enthusiasm for working a problem.

-------
                                   63
     The MPSP is developing a problem bank for grades 4-6.   One of the
criteria for selecting a problem for inclusion in the bank is that is
must be interesting to children.  Interest will be determined through
extensive interviewing and observing children as they solve problems.


     Question 7.  What problem solving strategies can children learn
to use effectively.?  In papers prepared for the MPSP, Greenes (1974)
and Seymour (1974) offer specific recommendations regarding skills and
strategies which should be taught.  Greenes not only listed several
strategies which can be taught to children in grades 4-6 but also made
suggestions for sequencing problem solving activities.  The skills and
strategies Greenes identified include:  estimate or guess, simplify, con-
duct an experiment, make a diagram, make a table, construct a graph,
write an equation, search for a pattern, construct a flowchart, partition
the decision space, and deductive logic.

     Seymour considers such skills as "making a table" and "constructing
a graph" as valuable aids to mathematical problem solving but would prob-
ably  classify such skills as substrategies because they are really tools
for applying a strategy.  The strategies he considers appropriate for the
intermediate grades include:  analogy, pattern recognition, deduction,
trial and error, organized listing, working backwards, combined strategies,
and  usual  strategies which are unique to a problem.

     The belief of mathematics educators like Greenes, Seymour, and Polya
(1957) that strategies can be taught should be given serious consider-
ation.  Most of our knowledge about learning and instruction is based on
the experiences of teachers who have thought long and hard about ways
to help children learn.  Although little research has been done on the
effectiveness of teaching problem solving strategies, the fact that
several master teachers are convinced of the feasibility of teaching
children the use of certain strategies should encourage teachers who are
planning to include problem solving as a part of their mathematics pro^
gram.


     Question 11.  Do any of the models of the problem solving process
adequately describe mathematical problem solving?  The primary purpose
of a model is to describe the salient and essential characteristics  of
the process or phenomenon which is being modeled.  Any model of the
problem solving process should be evaluated on the basis on the extent
to which it not only identifies the essential aspects of the process
but also the extent to which stages and relationships among those stages
are ident i f ied.

     An investigation of this question has evoked considerable inquiry
within the MPSP, and it is a major theme of this paper.  A discussion
of models of mathematical problem solving is included in a later  section
of this paper.

-------
                                     64
          The  Nature of the Mathematical Problem Solving Project
      The  Mathematical Problem Solving Project (MPSP), which is cosponsored
 by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics  and the Mathematics
 Education Development Center at Indiana University  and funded by  the
 National  Science Foundation, is working toward the  development of mathe-
 matical problem solving modules which can be  inserted into existing
 curriculum of grades 4-6.   Many types of problem situations will  be
 included  in these modules:   real-world applications of mathematics (i.e.,
 "real world" as the student sees it), problems related to the mathematics
 studies in the standard curriculum, mathematical recreations, and problems
 involving various strategies such as guess and test and pattern finding.
 While the MPSP is primarily a development project the materials being
 developed will be based upon research into the teaching and learning of
 problem solving and will be pilot tested in a number of elementary
 schools.

      The  project is in operation at three different centers:  the
 University of Northern Iowa, the Oakland Schools  (Pontiac, Michigan),
 and Indiana University. While the project has identified the central
 goal as being the development of problem solving modules for use  in
 grades 4-6, each center plays a distinct role.


 The Role  of_the University  of Northern  Iowa (U.N.I.) Center5

      The  MPSP site at the University of  Northern  Iowa is directed by
 George Immerzeel.   The primary role of the site is to develop a series
 of "skillsfl6  booklets and associated problem  solving experiences.
 Specifically,  the center at U.N.I,  is identifying the spectrum of required
 skills that are not part of the present  curriculum and writing materials
 that  build this spectrum for particular  problem solving strategies.

      After considering an extensive list of required problem solving
 skills and classifying these skills into those that are simple (require
 a  limited set  of tactics) and complex (requiring  a variety of tactics),
 seven were identified as appropriate  for students in grades 4 through 6:
           description summarizes the role of U.N.I, as reported by
George Immerzeel and his associates.

     Tliere is a semantics problem in trying to communicate ideas about
problem solving.  Terms like "skill," "strategy," "heuristics," and
"techniques" connote different things to different people.   The word
"skill," as used by the University of Northern Iowa  staff, refers to
generic problem solving techniques which are needed in order to use a
particular strategy.  Thus, "making a table" is a skill, whereas "pattern
finding" is a strategy.

-------
                                65


     1.  using an equation,

     2.  using a table,

     3.  using resources (reading, formulas, dictionaries, encyclopedias),

     4.  using a model (physical model, graph, picture, diagram),

     5.  make a simpler problem,

     6.  guess and test, and

     7.  compute to solve.

Each of these skills is simple in that they involve a single principle
tactic.  They do not depend upon an interrelation among tactics as is
the case in strategies such as pattern finding and goal stacking 7

     A "skills booklet" will be written for each of the seven skills.
These booklets will be designed to teach the subskills needed to use a
particular skill.  For example, for the Guess and Test Skills Booklet,
approximately 100 problems were written and the skills necessary to
solve the problems were identified.  These skills were then incorpo-
rated into the booklet.

     The skills booklet is written so that a student can use the booklet
independent of teacher input and also so the teacher can use the booklet
in a regular classroom setting.  After completing each booklet, the
student is given an evaluation that not only determines the student's
success in the skills but is a guide to group placement for the problem
solving experiences designed for the skills.

     The problem solving experiences consist of a set of cards for each
skill.  These cards represent five levels of difficulty and a variety of
interests.   Although a majority of the problems are supposed to have a
"real world" setting, there are also examples from all aspects of the
curriculum.  From this set of problems each student should be able to
find problems that not only fit her/his interests but also are at a level
of difficulty where the student will be challenged but have a reasonable
chance for success.  Also included in the problem set are problems in
which the use of the mini-calculator is appropriate.  These problems are
identified so the student knows the calculator is suggested for the prob*-
lem.   A separate skills booklet for the mini-calculator will be developed
which can be used with any type of problem solving strategy.

     As the skills booklets and problem sets are developed, they will be
field tested with students in grades 4-6 in the Malcolm Price Laboratory
School of the University of Northern Iowa.
      See Newell and Simon (1972) and Wickelgren (1974) for a description
of goal stacking.

-------
                                   66
The Role of  the  Oakland  Schools Center8

     David Wells is  the  director of the Oakland Schools Center.  This
center  is responsible for preparing teachers to field test and help
develop materials.   The  teachers will use their classrooms to field
test the materials developed at Oakland, U.N.I., and Indiana University.
Thus the Oakland Schools center operates the major field testing com-
ponents of the project.   Currently, there are twelve teachers partici-
pating  actively  in solving problems, discussing problem difficulty,
identifying  problem  solving strategies, developing problems for use  in
modules, and contributing to the development of modules.

     The participation of classroom teachers is an essential part  of
the project.  It is  also essential that these teachers teach
in a school  system which offers diverse socio-economic groupings of
children.  The Oakland Schools Center is ideally suited in this respect
since it has  approximately 260,000 students and 14,000 teachers and
contains industrialized  centers, surburban communities, and rural  areas.
 The  Role of  Indiana University (I.U.) Center

     The Mathematics Education Development Center, under the  direction
 of John LeBlanc,  is the third site involved in MPSP.  The  role  of the
 I.U. center  is twofold.  First, it is involved in the development of
 one  or more  modules based on information gathered through  work  with
-individual and small groups of students.  Second, the center  has major
 responsibility for evaluating the materials developed at the  other
 centers and  for making suggestions for revision.  At the same time, the
 staff of the Mathematics Education Development Center is best qualified
 among the three centers to conduct developmental research  into  the
 questions which will arise inevitably as the modules and problems are
 being created.  To date, research problems have been identified relate
 to problem difficulty and complexity and techniques for observing  and
 interviewing children as they attempt to solve problems.   The thrust of
 the  work of  the I.U. center will be discussed in more detail in a later
 section.
 Interrelationships of the MPSP Centers

      The roles of the three centers have been described briefly but the
 interrelationships among the centers has not been  specified.   Interaction
            description summarizes the  role  of  the  Oakland  Schools as
 reported by Stuart Choate,  Assistant Director  of the  Oakland Schools
 Center.

-------
                                   67
among the centers is determined on the basis of need for reaction to
ideas being investigated and materials being developed.  For example, it
is expected that materials devised by one center will be reacted to
by the other centers.  In this respect, there is a cyclic pattern of
continual development, testing, and evaluation of materials which are
produced (see Figure 1).  Also, all three centers will be involved in
identifying research-able issues for close scrutiny by the l.TJ. center.
             Develop

         problem solving

            materials
                                Evaluate

                                materials
         University of
         Northern Iowa
Oakland
Schools
                                 Indiana
                               University

Figure 1.  Interrelationship of primary roles of the MPSP centers.

     A final word should be said regarding the feasibility of a tri-
site project.  Such an organizational structure necessitates some
confusion, inefficiency, and duplication of efforts that must be taken
into account in assessing the project.  However, despite these short-
comings the tri-site aspect is viewed as a strength rather than a weak-
ness of the project.  The collaboration of educators with different

-------
                                   68
interests, experience, and expertise has been proposed by several leading
curriculum developers.  Having three centers offers a broader base  for
disseminating the materials which will be developed and provides a
wider range of expertise in the areas of teaching, materials development,
evaluation, and research.'
           Focus of Efforts in the MPSP at Indiana University

     This section is devoted to a description of the research and develop-
ment work at Indiana University during 1974-75.  Also, the current status
of the model of mathematical problem solving which is evolving will be
discussed.  Although the development of a model has been given little
direct attention during the past year, it seems appropriate to present
it in this paper in order to elicit the reader's reactions.

     The work of the I.U. center during the past year focused primarily
on intensive observation of students'  problem solving behavior, the
development of a problem bank, and the creation of a problem solving
module.  The details of each of these  three aspects of this work are
discussed in the sections which follow.
Observation of Fifth-Grade Students

     In order to get a better feeling for what types of problems students
find interesting and to investigate if students employ any discernible
strategies as they solve problems, the decision was made to spend some
time (approximately 6 weeks) observing fifth-grade children as they
attempted to solve problems without having any prior instruction.  Fifth-
graders were used because it seemed reasonable to fix the age level of
the children so that developmental factors related to age would not have
to be dealt with.

     Approximately eighty problems were found that were suitable for
most fifth-graders.  The problems were selected on the basis of:  rele-
vance to fifth-grade mathematics, potential interest for fifth-graders,
and "nonroutineness" (i.e., problems that are not standard textbook
"story problem").  Consideration also was given to selecting problems
which could be solved in more than one way.  Ten of the problems were
selected for use in interviewing students.
     9This view was articulated by James Gray who is the N.C.T.M.
representative on the MPSP Advisory Board.

-------
                                   69
     Two classes comprising approximately sixty fifth-grade students were
interviewed as they attempted to solve some of the ten problems.  The
first class of students was interviewed individually and in groups of
two, three, and four as they worked a set of four problems.  When groups
of students were interviewed, it proved too difficult to identify from
audio recordings the processes used by individuals.  Thus, all students
in the second class were interviewed individually.  The findings from
the interviews were:

     1.  Very few of the students wrote anything down.  Sorae drew
         a figure, but only after it was suggested by the interviewer.

     2.  Most students had difficulty retaining multiple conditions
         and considering two or more conditions at the same time.

     3.  Students often solved a problem that was not the stated
         problem.  They misread the problem or misinterpreted the
         problem.

     4.  Students in general did not use strategies, although a few
         attempted to identify patterns for some problems.

     The observation that many students were unable to coordinate multiple
conditions in a problem (finding 2) deserves elaboration.  One of the
problems presented to students was the following:

     There are 5 cups on the table.  John has 9 marbles, and he
     wants to put a different number of marbles under each cup.
     Can he do this?  Explain.

     There are three different conditions to coordinate:  five cups,
nine marbles, and a different number of marbles under each cup.   (Of
course, "John" cannot perform this task.)  Some students ignored the
third requirement and came up with 2, 2, 2, 2, 1 as their answer.  Other
students ignored the condition of having nine marbles and arrived at 4,
3, 2, 1, 0 for an answer.  It should be pointed out that although many
students did not initially coordinate all of the conditions, they were
able to do so after rereading the problem or being given a. simple clue
by the interviewer.  It should be added that it is possible that
students did not use all  of the conditions because they would not have
found a way to put the marbles under the cups otherwise.  It is likely
that they have been conditioned to find an "acceptable" answer at all
costs.  To them, getting an answer is the most important thing;  getting
an answer that makes sense is something else.  This situation is probably
not the fault of the students but the fault of a society which stresses
immediate results and values quanitity more than quality.
     Mini-Instruction of fifth-grade.  The results of the interviews
suggested that although the students were unsuccessful for a variety of
reasons, they did benefit from the questions asked and the hints given

-------
                                   70
by the interviewer.  Thus, it seemed feasible to devise short sequences
of instructional activities which would focus on helping children in the
areas that appeared to cause them the most trouble.

     A fifth-grade class, different from those interviewed, was divided
into four groups (3 groups of 8 children and 1 group of 7 children).  The
groups were approximately equal in ability based on  the scores from a
pretest on mathematical reasoning.  Each group was given forty-five minute
of instruction on each of four consecutive days.  The instruction varied
among groups by what was stressed.  The four different instructional
stresses were based on the findings from the interviews.  They were:

     1.  Using Strategies:          This group worked on using
                                    "pattern finding" and "simplifi-
                                    cation" in solving problems.

     2.  Coordinating Conditions:    This group considered the condi-
                                    tions of the problems and checked
                                    that the solution satisfied all of
                                    the conditions.

     3.  Understanding the Problem:  This group was given ways to help
                                    understand what  a problem is asking
                                    such as drawing  a figure or dis-
                                    tinguishing between relevant
                                    and irrelevant information.

     4.  Working Problems:          This group was given no particular
                                    instruction.  The students were
                                    given the problems and asked
                                    to work them.  They were told if
                                    they had the solutions right or
                                    wrong and given  hints when necessary.

     Each group was given nearly the same set of problems over the four-
day period.  These problems were selected because they were appropriate
for instruction in each group.   At the end of the four-day instructional
period, a posttest of four problems was given to all the students to see
if any change in their problem solving behavior had  occurred.  In addition,
two students from each group were individually interviewed as they worked
the posttest.

     There was no attempt to compare the groups statistically in terms of
problem solving performance.  This was not an experimental study to deter-
mine which of four instructional techniques was the  best, but rather an
exploratory investigation of the feasibility of providing instruction in
very specific aspects of the problem solving process.  As this point the
primary interest was to try out ideas in order to gain a narrower focus,

-------
                                   71
not to conduct careful planned and controlled experiments to test well-
formed hypotheses.

     The results of the mini-instruction were inconclusive.  Although
the group which received instruction on using strategies seemed to bene-
fit the most from the instruction, the teacher variable may well have
been the factor that caused this to happen since each group had a
different teacher.  In general, the extent of the influence of the small
group instructional sessions is unclear.  However, the insight gained
into the behavior of fifth-graders in small group problem solving situations
was invaluable.  Interviewing and observing students as they work on
mathematical problems has continued to be a primary activity at the I.U.
center.
Development of a Problem Bank and Problem Categorization Scheme

     The second major thrust of the I.U. center has been toward the
development of a large bank of problems of a wide variety of types.
As the size of this bank has grown, it has become necessary to determine
a scheme for categorizing the problems so that retrieval of problems
will be efficient.  A substantial effort has been undertaken to devise
a suitable categorization scheme.  In pursuit of this scheme the purposes
of having a problem bank had to be clarified.  The purposes of the prob-
lem bank are:

     1.  to provide classroom teachers with a source of problems
         of various types, and

     2.  to have available a wide range of problems with respect
         to structure and mathematical complexity, mathematical con-
         tent, problem setting, strategies used in solving the
         problems, interest, etc. for use in development of problem
         solving materials.

     One important use of the problem bank is as a source of problems
exemplifying a particular strategy.  For example, if a teacher wishes
to illustrate the use of the "pattern finding" strategy, he/she can go
to the problem bank and choose problems designated as "pattern finding"
problems.

     In order to categorize the problems in the bank four dimensions
were identified:  the setting of the problem, the complexity of the
problem, strategies applicable for a problem, and the mathematical
content of the problem.  Initial attempts to sort out the components
of each category resulted in the following outline for a categorization
scheme.

-------
                                72
  I.  The setting of problems
      A.   Verbal setting
          1.   Simple statement
          2.   Statement  In  story  form
          3.   Statement  in  game form
          4.   Statement  in  project form

      B.   Auxiliary nonverbal setting (a verbal  setting accom-
          panied by nonverbal information or materials  which are
          not essential  to  solving the problem)
          1.   Diagram/picture/graph
          2.   Concrete objects
          3.   Acting out the  problem
          4.   Hand calculators and other "facilitative" devices

      C.   Essential nonverbal setting (nonverbal information or
          materials essential to  solving the problem)
          1.   Diagram/picture/graph
          2.   Concrete objects
          3.   Acting out the  problem
          4.   Hand calculators and other "facilitative" devices

 II.  Complexity of problems
      A.   Complexity of  the problem  setting
          1.   Number of  words
          2.   Number of  conditions (numerical  and nonnumerical)
          3.   Type of connectives among conditions
          4.   Familiarity of  setting
          5.   Amount of  superfluous  information
          6.   Number of  clues provided  (verbal and nonverbal)

      B.   Complexity of  the solution process
          1.   Familiarity with the type of  solution
          2.   Number of  questions posed
          3.   Type of connectives among questions
          4.   Number of  variables
          5.   Type of connectives among variables
          6.   Number of  different operations  required
          7.   Type of operations  required
          8.   Number of  steps required to reach  solution

III.  Problem solving strategies
      A.   Pattern finding
      B.   Systeraatization
      C.   Visual perception
      D.   Inference
      E.   Trial-and-Error
      F.   Use and/or development  of  visual  aids
      G.   Use and/or development  of  simpler problems
      H.   Recall and use of previous experiences

-------
                                   73
    IV.  Mathematical content
         Since the problem bank will be used within the structure of
         the existing mathematics curriculum, the components of this
         category should be determined on the basis of topics
         included in various grade-five mathematics textbooks.

     Problems which exemplify the use of various strategies have not
been difficult to find.  Carole Greenes and Dale Seymour have provided
the MPSP with large collections of excellent problems which illustrate
particular strategies and which are appropriate for use in the inter-
mediate grades.  Complexity has proven to be the most challenging
category to consider.  Several weeks of intensive study resulted in a
revision of the outline related to the complexity of problems.  The
revised outline is presented here without discussion.  Work is now underway
to determine if factors included in this outline are critical in the
determination of problem complexity.

     I.  Complexity of problem statement
         A.  Vocabulary
             1.  Word frequency
             2.  Specialized use

         B.  Sentence factors (conceptualization of phrases)
             1.  Number of simple sentences
             2.  Average number of words per sentence
             3.  Decodability of phrases

         C.  Amount  of information
             1.  Numerals and symbols
             2.   Necessary  numerical  and nonnumerical data
             3.   Questions asked

         D.  Interest  factor
             1.   Number  of personal words
             2.   Number  of concrete nonmathematical words

    II.   Complexity  of the focusing process
         A.   Interrelationships  of conditions
             1.   Number  of bits  of irrelevant data
             2.   Types of connectives between conditions (and,  or,
                 if  .  .  .  ,  then)
             3.   Order of presentation of the givens and/or operations
             4.   Logical  structure  of the problem

        B.   Interrelationships  of  goals
             1.   Leading  questions
             2.   Corollary questions
             3.   Completely disjoint questions
             4.   Related  questions

-------
                                   74
   III.   Complexity of the solution process
         A.   Unique vs. non-unique vs.  no solution
         B.   Mathematical content involved
         C.   Types of strategies that could be used effectively
         D.   Minimum number of subgoals
         E.   Types of goals

    IV.   Complexity of evaluation
         A.   Ease of checking solution
         B.   Ease of generalizing solution
Module Development

     The development of instructional materials on pattern finding was
begun.  Pattern finding was chosen as the focus of the module because
the students had an accurate understanding of the word "pattern" and used
it in conversation.  Also, there is a wealth of problems which involve
pattern finding in their solutions.  Preliminary versions  of parts of the
module have been tested in fifth grade classrooms.  No formal evaluation
of the extent to which students learn to use a pattern finding strategy
has been conducted.  Instead, the testing has concentrated on readability
of the materials, clarity of presentation, format used, and interest
level.

     The attempt to develop a problem solving module on pattern finding
and determine a scheme for categorizing mathematical problems necessitated
a careful examination of the behaviors, both affective and cognitive,
which are demonstrated as a student tries to solve a problem.  This
analysis involved an attempt to determine a model of the problem solving
process which emphasizes the most important components of  the process and
provides an accurate description of how successful problem solvers think.
Toward a Model of the Problem Solving Process

     A search of the literature of problem solving revealed that several
attempts have been made to devise a model which describes problem solving.
It was appropriate to study some of these models in order to create a
model which approximates the process for solving mathematical problems.


     Dewey's model of reflective thinking.  In his classic book, How we
Think, Dewey proposes five phases of reflective thought (Dewey, 1933).
While reflective thought is not synonomous with problem solving, it is
clear that reflective thought is an essential part of problem solving.
The five phases are:  1.  suggestion,, direct action upon a situation is
inhibited thereby casuing conscious awareness of being "in a hole"
(p. 107);  2.  intellectualization, an intellectualization of the felt
difficulty leading to a definition of the problem; 3.  hypothesizing,
various hypotheses are identified to begin and guide observations in the
collection of factual material; 4.  reasoning, each hypothesis is mentally

-------
                                   75
elaborated upon through reasoning; and 5.  testing the hypothesis by action,
"some kind of testing by overt action to give experimental corroboration,
or verification, of the conjectural idea" (pp. 113-4).

     Dewey is careful to point out that these phases do not necessarily
follow one another in any set order.  This analysis is valuable in identi-
fying stages in reflective thinking and thus, in problem solving.  How-
ever, it considers only the logical aspects of reflective thought but does
not consider nonlogical "playfulness" or intuition.  It has been suggested
(Getzels, 1964) that Dewey's formal steps are more a statement of one
type of scientific method than an accurate description of how people
think.  As a result, this model of the process of solving problems may
describe how students ought to think, but it does not describe how stu-
dents usually do think when they are solving problems.


     Johnson's model of problem solving.  Whereas Dewey's model reflects
a logical analysis of problem solving, Johnson (1955) has provided an
analysis which is oriented to the psychological processes related to
problem solving.  Johnson's model is of particular interest because it
provides a framework in which "to interpret measures of problem difficulty
such as solution time" (cited in Bourne et al., 1971, p. 56).  Three
stages are included in his model:

     1.  preparation and orientation—the student gets an idea of
         what the problem involves;

     2.  production—the consideration of alternative approaches
         to a solution and the subsequent generation of possible
         solutions; and

     3.  judgment—the determination of the adequacy of a solution
         and the validity of the approach used to arrive at the
         solution.

     In addition to providing information about problem difficulty this
model offers a dimension that is not present in Dewey's model—it leads
to speculation about the effects of instruction.  In Johnson's model
preproduction activity by the problem solver is just as important as the
production stage.  Unfortunately, little is known about the preparation
state because researchers have preferred to investigate problem situations
which ate well-defined for the student.  Thus, the preparation stage plays
a less important role.  Future research efforts should include studies
which focus on the preparation stage of problem solving by examining
problems for which the student is not fully prepared.


     Polya's model of problem solving.  George Polya's extensive writings
have been a source of much valuable information regarding the problem of
teaching problem solving in mathematics  (Polya, 1957, 1962).  Unlike Dewey
and Johnson, Polya's concern lies primarily with mathematical problem

-------
                                 76
solving.  To him,  teaching  problem solving  involves  considerable  exper-
ience in solving problems and  serious  study of  the solution process.   The
teacher who wants  to  enhance her/his student's  ability  to solve problems
must direct the student's attention to certain  key questions and  sugges-
tions which correspond  to the  mental operations used to solve problems.
In order to group  these questions  in a convenient manner Polya suggests
four phases in the solution process:

     1.  understanding  the  problem,

     2.  devising  a plan,

     3.  carrying  out the plan,  and

     4.  looking back.

     Since Polya's four phases are familiar to  most mathematics educators
interested in mathematical  problem solving,  no  discussion of his model
will be presented  here.  It should be  pointed out that instead of being
a description of how  successful  problem solvers think, his model is a
proposal for teaching students how to  solve problems.  While this model
may be valuable as a  guide  in  organizing instruction  in problem solving,
it is too gross to be of much  help in  identifying potential areas of
difficulty for students or  clearly specifying the mental processes
involved in successful  problem solving.

     Webb's model  of  problem solving.  After reviewing the existing liter-
ature on mathematical problem  solving, Webb  (1974) devised a model which
is purported to be a  synthesis of  the  various models described in the
literature.  This  model contains three main stages in solving a problem:

     1.  preparation—includes defining and understanding the problem;
         understanding  what is unknown, what is given, and what the
         goals are;

     2.  production—includes  the  search for a path to attain the
         goals; recall  of principles,  facts, and rules from memory;
         generation of  new  concepts and rules to be used in solving
         the problem; and development of hypotheses and alternative
         plans that may lead to  one or more goals; and

     3.  Evaluation—includes  checking subgoals and the final solu-
         tion; and checking the  validity of procedures used during
         preparation  and production.

Webb stated that his model  "is not a hierarchial model in that preparation
always comes before production which always must precede evaluation.  This
is more a cyclic model"  (Webb, 1974, p. 4).   The models of Polya and Webb
have proven to be useful to the  staff at the Indiana University center of
the MPSP as rudimentary  models from which a more detailed and refined
model can be developed.

-------
                                77


      Some other models of problem solving.  In addition to the models
 proposed by Dewey, Johnson, Polya and Webb, at least two other thought-
 ful models have been developed.  The first is the model of Klausmeir
 and Goodwin (1966).  The major aspects of their model are highlighted
 below without discussion:

      1.  setting a goal,

      2.  appraising the situation,

      3.  trying to attain the goal,

      4.  confirming or rejecting a  solution,  and

      5.  reaching the goal.

      The major points of the second  model by  Wallas (1929)  are also high-
 lighted below without discussion:

      1.  preparation,

      2.   incubation (a mulling-over  period),

      3,   illumination (the  conception  of  a solution), and

      4.   verification,


A Working Model of  Problem Solving for the MPSP  at  Indiana  University

      The  primary  limitation  of  each  of the models that have been discussed
is that  they are  either prescriptive (viz., Dewey and Polya) or
only  grossly descriptive  (viz., Johnson,  Klausmeir  and Goodwin, Wallas,
and Webb).  The prescriptive models  suggest techniques to help the  student
to be a better problem solver.  The  descriptive models may  be more  valu-
able  in the sense that they  identify phases the  student goes through
during  problem solving.  A goal of the MPSP is to devise a more detailed
and refined descriptive model.

     The  search for such a model has led  to an investigation of informa-
tion processing approaches to problem solving research.   With the possible
exception of gestalt psychology, information processing theory seems to
be the only psychological theory which has problem solving as a central
focus.  A primary thrust of information processing theory is to develop
a description of specific types of problems that is precise enough  to
enable an explanation of problem solving behavior in terms of basic cog-
nitive processes.   The most complete description of information process-
ing theory has been presented by Newell and Simon (1972).   Wickelgren
(1974) has attempted to develop an operationalized theory of problem
solving by combining elements of information processing  theory and the
ideas of master teachers like George Polya.

-------
                               78
     The work of'Newell and Simon, and Wickelgren has led the author to
the model for solving mathematics problems which is described in the
following paragraphs.  This model is,  of course, not as refined as it
should be nor does it necessarily generalize to all types of successful
mathematical problem solving behavior.  However, it does pinpoint some
critical components of problem solving behavior which are missing in
the other models.  Six distinct, but not necessarily disjoint, stages
are included in this model:

     1.  problem awareness,

     2.  problem comprehension,

     3.  goal analysis,

     4.  plan development,

     5.  plan implementation, and

     6.  procedure and solution evaluation.

It should be emphasized that these stages are not necessarily sequential.
In fact it only rarely happens that these stages do occur sequentially
and distinctly from each other.

     In keeping with an information processing approach to building a
model, it would be desirable to devise a flow chart that would describe
the student's cognitive processes as progress is made from Problem Aware-
ness through Procedure and Solution Evaluation.  However, since the
stages are not hierarchically ordered or even distinct, for most problems
it is not possible to devise a completely accurate diagram of the flow of
progress during problem solving.  Figure 3 (page 82) is a rough descrip-
tion of the way in which the stages of the model are related.

     Stage 1:  Problem awareness.  A situation is posed for the student.
Before this situation becomes a problem for the student, he/she must
realize that a difficulty exists.  A difficulty must exist in the sense
that the student must recognize that the situation cannot be resolved
readily.  This recognition often follows from initial failure to attain
a goal.  This view of what constitutes a problem is consistent with
Bourne's description of a problem situation as one in which initial
attempts fail to accomplish some goal (Bourne et al., 1971).  A second
component of the awareness stage is the student's willingness to try to
solve the problem.  If the student either does not recognize a diffi-
culty or is not willing to proceed in trying to solve the problem, it is
meaningless to proceed  (see Figure 2),

-------
                                79
     Problem
      posed
                                                     Stop
                                                      or
                                                    repose
                Is
             aware of
            difficulty?
                        Can
                     difficulty
                    be resolved?
    willin
to attempt a
 solution?
                                                          S
                                                       Decides
                                                        to work
  Stop
   or
repose the
 problem
      Figure 2:  Schematic representation of problem awareness.
     Stage 2;  Problem comprehension.  Once the student is aware of the
problem situation and declares a willingness to eliminate it as a problem,
the task of making sense out of the problem begins.  This stage involves
at least two sub-stages:  translation and internalization.  Translation
involves interpretation of the information the problem provides into
terms which have meaning for the student.  Internalization requires that
the problem solver sort out the relevant information and determine how
this information interrelates.  Most importantly, this stage results in
the formation of some sort of internal representation of the problem
within the problem solver.  This representation may not be accurate at
first (or it may never be accurate, hence the student fails to solve the
problem), but it furnishes the student with a means of establishing goals
or priorities for working on the problem.  It is here that the nonsequen-
tial nature of the model shows up for the first time.  The accuracy of
the problem solver's internal representation may increase as progress is
made toward a solution.  Thus, the degree of problem comprehension will
be a factor in several stages of the solution process.

     Stage 3:  Goal analysis.  It seems that the problem solver may jump
back and forth from this stage to another.  For some problems it is
appropriate to establish subgoals, for others subgoals are not needed.
It is often true that the identification and subsequent attainment of
a subgoal aids both problem comprehension and procedure development.

-------
                               80
     Goal analysis can be viewed as an attempt to reformulate the problem
so that familiar strategies and techniques can be used.  It may also
involve an identification of the component parts of a problem.  It is
a process which moves from the goal itself backwards in order to separate
the different components of the problem.  Thus, goal analysis actually
includes more than a simple specification of given information, specifi-
cation of the relationships among the information, and specification of
the operations which may be needed (see Resnick & Glaser, 1976, for a
more detailed discussion of goal analysis).

     Stage 4:  Plan development.  It is during this stage that the pro-
blem solver gives conscious attention to devising a plan of attack.
Developing a plan involves much more than identifying potential strate-
gies (e.g., pattern finding and solving a simpler related problem).  It
also includes ordering subgoals and specifying the operations which may
be used.  It is perhaps this stage more than any other that causes diffi-
culty for students.  It is common to hear mathematics students proclaim
after watching their teacher work a problem:  "How did he ever think of
that?  I never would have thought of that trick."  The main sources of
difficulty in learning how to formulate a plan of attack emanate from
the fact that students are prone to give up if a task cannot be done
easily.  Of course, if problems can be done too easily, they  are not
really problems.  A good problem causes initial failure which too often
results in a refusal to continue.  This state of affairs is not the fault
of students, but rather the fault of teachers who do not recognize that
initial failure is a necessary condition for problem solving (Shumway,
1974).  It may also be true that students are unable to devise good plans
because they have few plans at their disposal.  There is preliminary evi-
dence from work done at the Indiana University center of MPSP that many
children in grades 4-6 proceed primarily in a trial-and-error fashion
until they either find a "solution" that satisfies them or give up.
Equipping students of this age with a few well-chosen strategies may
facilitate their ability to plan.

     Another source of difficulty for students at this stage is in order-
ing subgoals and specifying the operations to be used.  For many students
the hardest part of problem solving lies with knowing what to do first
and organizing their ideas.  Consequently, in addition to teaching stu-
dents strategies, attention must be given to helping them organize their
thinking and planning.

     Stage 5:  Plan implementation.  At this stage, the problem solver
tries out a plan which has been devised.  The possibility that executive
errors may arise confounds the situation at this stage.  The student who
correctly decides to make a table and look for a pattern may fail to see
the pattern due to a simple computation error.  Errors of this type prob-
ably  cannot be eliminated, but they can be reduced if instruction on
implementing a plan also considers the importance of evaluating the plan
while it is being tried.  Thus, while stages 5 and 6 are distinct, they
are not disjoint.  The main dangers of stage 5 are that the problem
solvers may forget the plan, become confused as the plan is carried out,

-------
                                81


 or  be unable to fit  together  the various  parts of  the plan.   Fitting
 together  the parts of  a plan  can be  a very  difficult task in  itself.
 This  difficulty may  arise  from  the fact that  the best sequencing of
 steps in  the plan or the best ordering of subgoals may not be clear
 to  the problem solver.   For some problems the sequencing  of subgoals
 does  not  matter, while for others it is essential  that subgoals be
 achieved  in  a particular order.  The reader is referred to Chapter 6
 of  Wickelgren's book How To Solve Problems  for an  in-depth analysis
 of  techniques for defining subgoals  and using them to solve problems
 (Wickelgren,  1974).

      Stage 6:   Procedures  and solution evaluation^  Successful problem
 solving usually is the result of systematic evaluation of  the appro-
 priateness of the decisions made during the problem solving and thought-
 ful examination of the results obtained.  The role of evaluation in
 problem solving goes far beyond simply checking the answer to be sure
 that  it makes sense.   Instead, it is an ongoing process that  starts as
 soon  as the  problem  solver begins goal analysis and continues long after
 a solution has  been  found.  Procedure and solution evaluation may be
 viewed as a  process  of seeking answers to certain  questions as the
 problem solver  works on a  problem.  Representative of the  questions
 which should  be asked  by the  problem solver at each stage  are the
 following:

      1.   problem comprehension stage—What  are the relevant and irrel-
          evant  data  involved  in the problem?  Do I understand the
          relationships  among  the information  given?  Do I  understand
          the meaning of  all the terms that  are involved?

      2.   goal analysis  stage—Are there any subgoals which may help
         me achieve  the  goal?  Can these subgoals  be ordered?  Is my
          ordering of subgoals correct?  Have  I correctly identified
          the  conditions  operating in the problem?

      3.   plan development  stage—Is there more than one way to do
          this problem?   Is there a best way?  Have I ever solved a
          problem like  this one before?  Will  the plan lead to the
          goal or a subgoal?

     4.  plan implementation  stage—Am I using this strategy correctly?
          Is the  ordering of the steps in my plan appropriate,
         or could I have used a different  ordering?

     5.  solution evaluation  stage—Is my  solution generalizable?
         Does my solution  satisfy all the  conditions of the prob~
          lem?   What have I learned that will help me solve other
         problems?

     Figure 3 attempts to illustrate the interrelationships that exist
among the stages in the model.  It also suggests how a student might
proceed in solving a problem.

-------
                              82
             Problem
              Posed
            Awareness
             Attained
             Problem
          Comprehension
Procedure
Evaluation
              Solution
             Evaluation

   Figure  3:   Schematic  representation  of a model of mathematical problem
              solving.
     How the model may be used.  The most valuable aspect of this model
is that it provides a conceptual framework for identifying the factors
which most influence success in problem solving.   This framework can be
useful to the teacher who is trying to organize appropriate problem
solving experiences for students by highlighting various potential
sources of difficulty for problem solvers.  It also emphasizes that

-------
                               83
teachers cannot be content to teach students how to solve problems by
simply showing a few "tricks of the trade."  Of course, the model does
not describe problem solving for all types of problems and, in this
sense at least, it is incomplete.  But, it does supply a partial
explication of a theory of problem solving which, although not fully
conceptualized, is being created.  The development of a theory of pro-
blem solving will give direction and add focus to any research efforts.
Such a theory is needed critically within mathematics education at the
present time.  Many of the research efforts in mathematical problem
solving which have been conducted were well-conceived and carefully
done, but the results of these efforts have had little impact on
instructional practice.  This is partially due to the diversity of
types of research and the conflicting results which have been obtained.
It is also due to the fact that none of the results seem to be general-
ized to all types of mathematical problems.  It may be that no single
theory, and hence no single model, can accurately depict problem solving
for all types of problems and all types of problem solvers.  Even with
the possibility of such a state of affairs, it is worthwhile to continue
the search for a suitable model since such a search will provide valuable
information about the nature of the problem solving process.
                      Plans For Future Research
     Although the MPSP is primarily a development project, an investiga-
tion of a few research questions will be included as a part of the efforts
during 1975-76.  Much of the work done at I.U. during the past year can be
classified as exploratory.  Emphasis was placed on intensive observation
of students, the collection of problems, the creation of a problem solving
module, and the design of a suitable model for mathematical problem solv-
ing.  While none of these endeavors can be considered research in the
usual sense, all of the work at I.U. was conducted with a research spirit.
That is, every effort was made to approach each issue in an open-minded
and objective manner and to apply the scientific method of inquiry.

     Perhaps the most valuable result of the work at the I.U. center was
the identification of three areas within the problem solving process which
cause difficulty for fifth-graders.  Two of these difficulties are related
to problem comprehension, while the third is related to plan development
and implementation.

     1.  Students often misread or misinterpreted problems.

     2.  Students had difficulty retaining and coordinating
         multiple conditions in a problem.

     3.  Students do not appear to use any strategies during
         problem solving.

-------
                               84
Further investigation of the first difficulty suggested that students
often perceive a simplified version of a stated problem.   The students
then proceed to solve the problem as they perceive it.  In a few cases,
the students were not even aware that a problem existed.   In other
cases students had trouble understanding phrases in problems (e.g., "a
checker in every row and in every column" and "every sixth night").
Clearly, students cannot solve problems they don't fully understand.
It is important, then, to pay special attention to the factors which
influence problem comprehension.  More specifically, it is important
to determine the primary determinants of reading difficulty since most
mathematical problems are presented in a written form.

     Several measures of comprehension of written passages have been
developed by reading specialists.  However, there is reason to believe
that these measures may not be appropriate for written mathematical
passages since mathematical English appears to be much different from
ordinary English.  Kane (1968) has suggested that there are at least
four differences between mathematical English and ordinary English:
(a) redundancies of letters, word, and syntax are different, (b) names
of mathematical objects usually have a single denotation; (c) adjec-
tives are more important in mathematical English than in ordinary
English; and (d) the grammar and syntax of mathematical English are
less flexible than in ordinary English.

     If mathematical English is significantly different from ordinary
English, it is essential that the nature of these differences be deter-
mined.  Two members of the MPSP staff at I.U., Norman Webb and Barbara
Moses, have designed a study which aims at identifying a reliable and
accurate measure of comprehension of written mathematics problems.
Their study will investigate the following questions:

     1.  Is the Cloze procedure   a reliable measure of comprehen-
         sion for individual mathematical problems?

     2.  What is the relationship of certain stimulus measures of
         mathematical problem statements to the mean Cloze score
         percentage?

     3.  What stimulus measures are  the best predictors of mean
         Cloze score percentage?

     Stimulus measures will  include  such variables  as  the number  of one-
syllable words, nouns, personal words,  symbols connectives,  sentences,
and  clauses per  100 words  as well as  the number of  words with  special-
ized mathematical meanings and  the average sentence length.
       The Cloze procedure  is a popular  technique  for measuring reada-
 bility of long passages.  The procedure  Involves deleting every nth
 word or  symbol of  a  passage and replacing  them with blanks.  The  student
 must fill in  the blanks.  The score  is determined  by the number of
 responses matching the deleted material.   A high score  indicates  high
 readability.

-------
                                 85
     Webb and Moses expect that one or two stimulus measures will be
found that can be used to predict the difficulty of comprehending a
mathematical problem.  They also expect the Cloze procedure to prove
to be an adequate measure of readability for mathematical problems.
If such expectations are supported, the task of classifying problems
according to complexity will be greatly reduced.

     The fact that many of the fifth-graders were unable to coordinate
and retain the conditions given in a problem has led to the design of
a study to investigate particular issues related to this fact.  Another
MPSP staff member, Fadia Harik, has decided to explore the influence
the number of conditions in a problem has on success in solving prob-
lems.  In addition, she will investigate the effect certain types of
teacher clues has on problem solving success.  This aspect of her study
arose from the observation that although fifth-graders do not initially
coordinate multiple conditions simultaneously, they are able to do so
in some problems if the teacher provides clues or asks the students to
reread the problem.H

     Research studies like those of Webb and Moses, and Harik have been
carefully conceived, organized, and planned.  Their questions have
risen from a concern for developing a sensible theory of mathematical
problem solving.  It is only by conducting research based on a sound
conceptual framework that any significant progress will be made toward
developing instructional materials which will enhance children's
ability to solve mathematical problems.
       Both the study by Harik and the one by Webb and Moses have been
completed since this paper was written.  The interested reader can
obtain information about the results of these studies by contacting
the author of this paper.

-------
                                 86
                            References
 Bourne, L. E.,  & Dominowskl, R. L.  Thinking.  Annual Review of  Psychology,
      1972, 23,  105-130.	

 Bourne, L. E.,  Ekstrand, B. R., & Dominowski, R. L.  The psychology  of
      thinking.  Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey:  Prentice-Hall, 1971.

 Davis, G. A.  Current status of research and theory in human problem
      solving.   Psychological Bulletin. 1966, £6(1), 36-54.

 Dewey, J.  How  we think.  Boston:  D. C. Heath & Co., 1933.

 Duncker, K.  On problem-solving.  Psychological Monographs. 1945, 58(5,
     Whole No.  270).

 Gagne, R. M.  The conditions of learning (2nd ed.).  New York:  Holt,
     Rinehart & Winston, 1970.

 Getzels, J. W.  Creative thinking, problem-solving, and instruction.  In
     H. G. Richey (Ed.), Theories of learning and instruction, Part  I.
     Chicago:  The National Society for the Study of Education, 1964.

 Green, B. F.  Introduction:  Current trends in problem-solving.  In  B.
     Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem solving:  Research, methods and theory.
     New York:  John Wiley, 1966.

 Greenes, C.  Untitled paper prepared for the Mathematical Problem Solving
     Project, 1974 (available in mineograph form only).

 Henderson, K. B., & Pingry, R. E.  Problem-solving in mathematics.   In
     H. F. Fehr (Ed.), The learning of mathematics its theory and prac-
     tice. Twenty-first Yearbook.  Washington, D.C.:  National Council
     of Teachers of Mathematics, 1953, 228-270.

Johnson, D. M.  The psychology of thought and judgment.   New York:  Harper
     & Row, 1955.

Kane, R. B.  The readability of mathematics books.   Journal of Research
     in Science Teaching. 1968, _5, 196-298.

Kendler, H. H.,  & Kendler,  T.  S.  Vertical and horizontal processes in
     problem solving.  Psychological Review. 1962,  69,  1-16.

Kilpatrick, J.  Problem solving and creative behavior in mathematics.  In
     J. W. Wilson and L.  R.  Carry (Eds.),  Reviews of Recent Research in
     Mathematics Education.   Studies in mathematics  (Vol.  19).   Stanford,
     California:  School Mathematics Study Group, 1969,  153-187.

-------
                                87
Klausmier, H. J., & Goodwin, W.  Learning and human abilities:  Educa-
     tional psychology (2nd ed,).  New York:  Harper & Row, 1966.

Maier, N. R. F.  Problem solving and creativity in individuals and
     groups.  Belmont, California:  Brooks-Cole, 1970.

Maltzman, I.  Thinking:  From a behavioristic point of view.  Psycho-
     logical Review, 1955, j>6, 367-386.

Newell, A., & Simon, H. A.  Human problem solving.  Englewood Cliffs,
     New Jersey:  Prentice-Hall, 1972.

Polya, G.  Mathematical discovery (Vol. 1).  New York:  John Wiley,
     1962.

Polya, G.  Presentation as a part of a panel discussion on mathematical
     problem solving.  Annual Meeting of the American Mathematical
     Society, San Francisco, January 15-18, 1974.

Resnick, L. B., & Glaser, R.  Problem solving and intelligence.  In
     L. B. Resnick (Ed.), The nature of intelligence.  New York:
     Halsted Press, 1976.

Seymour, D.  Considerations for a mathematics problem-solving curricu-
     lum for the intermediate grades.  Paper prepared for the Mathe-
     matical Problem Solving Project, 1974 (available in mimeograph
     form only).

Shumway, R. J.  Untitled paper prepared for the Mathematical Problem
     Solving Project, 1974 (available in mimeograph form only).

Wallas, G.  The art of thought.  New York:  Harcourt, Brace, 1926.

Webb, N.  A review of the literature related to problem-solving tasks
     and problem-solving strategies used by students in grades 4, 5
     and 6.  Progress report prepared for the Advisory Board of the
     Mathematical Problem Solving Project, 1974 (available in mimeo-
     graph form only).

Wertheimer, M.  Productive thinking.  New York:  Harper & Row, 1959.

Wickelgren, W. A.  How to solve problems.   San Francisco:  W. H. Freeman,
     1974.

-------
                                   89
                               Participants
  1.   Jeffrey  Charles Barnett




  2.   Morris I. Beers




  3.   David Bradbard




  4.   Nicholas A. Branca




  5.   Sandi Clarkson




  6.   Mary Kay Corbitt




  7.   Thomas J. Cooney




  8.   Carolyn Ehr




  9.   Beverly Gimmes tad




10.   Dorothy Goldberg




11.   Gerald A. Goldin




12.   Douglas A. Grouws




13.  Fadia Harik




14.  Larry L.  Hatfield




15.  Ruth E.  Heintz




16.  Mary Grace Kantowski




17.  Howard M. Kellogg




18.  Jeremy Kilpatrick
 19.  Gerald Kulm




 20.  Kil  S. Lee




 21.  Richard A.  Lesh




 22.  Frank K. Lester, Jr,




 23.  John F. Lucas




 24.  C. Edwin McClintock




 25.  Len  Pikaart




 26.  Sid  Rachlin




 27.  H. Radatz




 28.  Danielle Scheier




 29.  John Schleup




 30.  Ed Silver




 31.  J. Phillip  Smith




 32.  Larry Sowder




 33.  Leslie P. Steffe




 34.  Kenneth E.  Vos




 35.  Norman L. Webb




36.  Arvum Weinzweig




37.  James W.  Wilson

-------