The Optimism Bias by Tali Sharot: extract

Our brains may be hardwired to look on the bright side, says neuroscientist Tali Sharot in this extract from her new book

Tali Sharot, author of The Optimism Bias
Neuroscientist Tali Sharot, author of The Optimism Bias, December 2011. Photograph: Andy Hall/Observer New Review

We like to think of ourselves as rational creatures. We watch our backs, weigh the odds, pack an umbrella. But both neuroscience and social science suggest that we are more optimistic than realistic. On average, we expect things to turn out better than they wind up being. People hugely underestimate their chances of getting divorced, losing their job or being diagnosed with cancer; expect their children to be extraordinarily gifted; envision themselves achieving more than their peers; and overestimate their likely life span (sometimes by 20 years or more).

  1. The Optimism Bias: Why we're wired to look on the bright side
  2. by Tali Sharot
  3. Buy it from the Guardian bookshop
  1. Tell us what you think: Star-rate and review this book

The belief that the future will be much better than the past and present is known as the optimism bias. It abides in every race, region and socioeconomic bracket. Schoolchildren playing when-I-grow-up are rampant optimists, but so are grown-ups: a 2005 study found that adults over 60 are just as likely to see the glass half full as young adults.

You might expect optimism to erode under the tide of news about violent conflicts, high unemployment, tornadoes and floods and all the threats and failures that shape human life. Collectively we can grow pessimistic – about the direction of our country or the ability of our leaders to improve education and reduce crime. But private optimism, about our personal future, remains incredibly resilient. A survey conducted in 2007 found that while 70% thought families in general were less successful than in their parents' day, 76% of respondents were optimistic about the future of their own family.

Overly positive assumptions can lead to disastrous miscalculations – make us less likely to get health checkups, apply sunscreen or open a savings account, and more likely to bet the farm on a bad investment. But the bias also protects and inspires us: it keeps us moving forward rather than to the nearest high-rise ledge. Without optimism, our ancestors might never have ventured far from their tribes and we might all be cave dwellers, still huddled together and dreaming of light and heat.

To make progress, we need to be able to imagine alternative realities – better ones – and we need to believe that we can achieve them. Such faith helps motivate us to pursue our goals. Optimists in general work longer hours and tend to earn more. Economists at Duke University found that optimists even save more. And although they are not less likely to divorce, they are more likely to remarry – an act that is, as Samuel Johnson wrote, the triumph of hope over experience.

Even if that better future is often an illusion, optimism has clear benefits in the present. Hope keeps our minds at ease, lowers stress and improves physical health. Researchers studying heart-disease patients found that optimists were more likely than non-optimistic patients to take vitamins, eat low-fat diets and exercise, thereby reducing their overall coronary risk. A study of cancer patients revealed that pessimistic patients under 60 were more likely to die within eight months than non-pessimistic patients of the same initial health, status and age.

In fact, a growing body of scientific evidence points to the conclusion that optimism may be hardwired by evolution into the human brain. The science of optimism, once scorned as an intellectually suspect province of pep rallies and smiley faces, is opening a new window on the workings of human consciousness. What it shows could fuel a revolution in psychology, as the field comes to grips with accumulating evidence that our brains aren't just stamped by the past. They are constantly being shaped by the future.

Hardwired for hope?

I would have liked to tell you that my work on optimism grew out of a keen interest in the positive side of human nature. The reality is that I stumbled onto the brain's innate optimism by accident. After living through 9/11, in New York City, I had set out to investigate people's memories of the terrorist attacks. I was intrigued by the fact that people felt their memories were as accurate as a videotape, while often they were filled with errors. A survey conducted around the country showed that 11 months after the attacks, individuals' recollections of their experience that day were consistent with their initial accounts (given in September 2011) only 63% of the time. They were also poor at remembering details of the event, such as the names of the airline carriers. Where did these mistakes in memory come from?

Scientists who study memory proposed an intriguing answer: memories are susceptible to inaccuracies partly because the neural system responsible for remembering episodes from our past might not have evolved for memory alone. Rather, the core function of the memory system could in fact be to imagine the future – to enable us to prepare for what has yet to come. The system is not designed to perfectly replay past events, the researchers claimed. It is designed to flexibly construct future scenarios in our minds. As a result, memory also ends up being a reconstructive process, and occasionally, details are deleted and others inserted.

To test this, I decided to record the brain activity of volunteers while they imagined future events – not events on the scale of 9/11, but events in their everyday lives – and compare those results with the pattern I observed when the same individuals recalled past events. But something unexpected occurred. Once people started imagining the future, even the most banal life events seemed to take a dramatic turn for the better. Mundane scenes brightened with upbeat details as if polished by a Hollywood script doctor. You might think that imagining a future haircut would be pretty dull. Not at all. Here is what one of my participants pictured: "I was getting my hair cut to donate to Locks of Love [a charity that fashions wigs for young cancer patients]. It had taken me years to grow it out, and my friends were all there to help celebrate. We went to my favourite hair place in Brooklyn and then went to lunch at our favourite restaurant."

I asked another participant to imagine a plane ride. "I imagined the takeoff – my favourite! – and then the eight-hour-long nap in between and then finally landing in Krakow and clapping the pilot for providing the safe voyage," she responded. No tarmac delays, no screaming babies. The world, only a year or two into the future, was a wonderful place to live in.

If all our participants insisted on thinking positively when it came to what lay in store for them personally, what does that tell us about how our brains are wired? Is the human tendency for optimism a consequence of the architecture of our brains?

The Human time machine

To think positively about our prospects, we must first be able to imagine ourselves in the future. Optimism starts with what may be the most extraordinary of human talents: mental time travel, the ability to move back and forth through time and space in one's mind. Although most of us take this ability for granted, our capacity to envision a different time and place is in fact critical to our survival.

It is easy to see why cognitive time travel was naturally selected for over the course of evolution. It allows us to plan ahead, to save food and resources for times of scarcity and to endure hard work in anticipation of a future reward. It also lets us forecast how our current behaviour may influence future generations. If we were not able to picture the world in a hundred years or more, would we be concerned with global warming? Would we attempt to live healthily? Would we have children?

While mental time travel has clear survival advantages, conscious foresight came to humans at an enormous price – the understanding that somewhere in the future, death awaits. Ajit Varki, a biologist at the University of California, San Diego, argues that the awareness of mortality on its own would have led evolution to a dead end. The despair would have interfered with our daily function, bringing the activities needed for survival to a stop. The only way conscious mental time travel could have arisen over the course of evolution is if it emerged together with irrational optimism. Knowledge of death had to emerge side by side with the persistent ability to picture a bright future.

The capacity to envision the future relies partly on the hippocampus, a brain structure that is crucial to memory. Patients with damage to their hippocampus are unable to recollect the past, but they are also unable to construct detailed images of future scenarios. They appear to be stuck in time. The rest of us constantly move back and forth in time; we might think of a conversation we had with our spouse yesterday and then immediately of our dinner plans for later tonight.

But the brain doesn't travel in time in a random fashion. It tends to engage in specific types of thoughts. We consider how well our kids will do in life, how we will obtain that sought-after job, afford that house on the hill and find perfect love. We imagine our team winning the crucial game, look forward to an enjoyable night on the town or picture a winning streak at the blackjack table. We also worry about losing loved ones, failing at our job or dying in a terrible plane crash – but research shows that most of us spend less time mulling over negative outcomes than we do over positive ones. When we do contemplate defeat and heartache, we tend to focus on how these can be avoided.

Findings from a study I conducted a few years ago with prominent neuroscientist Elizabeth Phelps suggest that directing our thoughts of the future toward the positive is a result of our frontal cortex's communicating with subcortical regions deep in our brain. The frontal cortex, a large area behind the forehead, is the most recently evolved part of the brain. It is larger in humans than in other primates and is critical for many complex human functions such as language and goal setting. Using a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanner, we recorded brain activity in volunteers as they imagined specific events that might occur to them in the future. Some of the events that I asked them to imagine were desirable (a great date or winning a large sum of money), and some were undesirable (losing a wallet, ending a romantic relationship). The volunteers reported that their images of sought-after events were richer and more vivid than those of unwanted events.

This matched the enhanced activity we observed in two critical regions of the brain: the amygdala, a small structure deep in the brain that is central to the processing of emotion, and the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), an area of the frontal cortex that modulates emotion and motivation. The rACC acts like a traffic conductor, enhancing the flow of positive emotions and associations. The more optimistic a person was, the higher the activity in these regions was while imagining positive future events (relative to negative ones) and the stronger the connectivity between the two structures.

The findings were particularly fascinating because these precise regions – the amygdala and the rACC – show abnormal activity in depressed individuals. While healthy people expect the future to be slightly better than it ends up being, people with severe depression tend to be pessimistically biased: they expect things to be worse than they end up being. People with mild depression are relatively accurate when predicting future events. They see the world as it is. In other words, in the absence of a neural mechanism that generates unrealistic optimism, it is possible all humans would be mildly depressed.

Can optimism change reality?

The problem with pessimistic expectations, such as those of the clinically depressed, is that they have the power to alter the future; negative expectations shape outcomes in a negative way. How do expectations change reality?

To answer this question my colleague, cognitive neuroscientist Sara Bengtsson, devised an experiment in which she manipulated positive and negative expectations of students while their brains were scanned and tested their performance on cognitive tasks. To induce expectations of success, she primed college students with words such as smart, intelligent and clever just before asking them to perform a test. To induce expectations of failure, she primed them with words like stupid and ignorant. The students performed better after being primed with an affirmative message.

Examining the brain-imaging data, Bengtsson found that the students' brains responded differently to the mistakes they made depending on whether they were primed with the word clever or the word stupid. When the mistake followed positive words, she observed enhanced activity in the anterior medial part of the prefrontal cortex (a region that is involved in self-reflection and recollection). However, when the participants were primed with the word stupid, there was no heightened activity after a wrong answer. It appears that after being primed with the word stupid, the brain expected to do poorly and did not show signs of surprise or conflict when it made an error.

A brain that doesn't expect good results lacks a signal telling it, "Take notice – wrong answer!" These brains will fail to learn from their mistakes and are less likely to improve over time. Expectations become self-fulfilling by altering our performance and actions, which ultimately affects what happens in the future. Often, however, expectations simply transform the way we perceive the world without altering reality itself. Let me give you an example. While writing these lines, my friend calls. He is at Heathrow waiting to get on a plane to Austria for a skiing holiday. His plane has been delayed for three hours already, because of snowstorms at his destination. "I guess this is both a good and bad thing," he says.

Waiting at the airport is not pleasant, but he quickly concludes that snow today means better skiing conditions tomorrow. His brain works to match the unexpected misfortune of being stuck at the airport to its eager anticipation of a fun getaway.

A cancelled flight is hardly tragic, but even when the incidents that befall us are the type of horrific events we never expected to encounter, we automatically seek evidence confirming that our misfortune is a blessing in disguise. No, we did not anticipate losing our job, being ill or getting a divorce, but when these incidents occur, we search for the upside. These experiences mature us, we think. They may lead to more fulfilling jobs and stable relationships in the future. Interpreting a misfortune in this way allows us to conclude that our sunny expectations were correct after all – things did work out for the best.

The role of the caudate nucleus

How do we find the silver lining in storm clouds? To answer that, my colleagues – renowned neuroscientist Ray Dolan and neurologist Tamara Shiner – and I instructed volunteers in the fMRI scanner to visualise a range of medical conditions, from broken bones to Alzheimer's, and rate how bad they imagined these conditions to be. Then we asked them: If you had to endure one of the following, which would you rather have – a broken leg or a broken arm? Heartburn or asthma? Finally, they rated all the conditions again. Minutes after choosing one particular illness out of many, the volunteers suddenly found that the chosen illness was less intimidating. A broken leg, for example, may have been thought of as "terrible" before choosing it over some other malady. However, after choosing it, the subject would find a silver lining: "With a broken leg, I will be able to lie in bed watching TV, guilt-free."

In our study, we also found that people perceived adverse events more positively if they had experienced them in the past. Recording brain activity while these reappraisals took place revealed that highlighting the positive within the negative involves, once again, a tête-à-tête between the frontal cortex and subcortical regions processing emotional value. While contemplating a mishap, like a broken leg, activity in the rACC modulated signals in a region called the striatum that conveyed the good and bad of the event in question – biasing activity in a positive direction.

It seems that our brain possesses the philosopher's stone that enables us to turn lead into gold and helps us bounce back to normal levels of wellbeing. It is wired to place high value on the events we encounter and put faith in its own decisions. This is true not only when forced to choose between two adverse options (such as selecting between two courses of medical treatment) but also when we are selecting between desirable alternatives. Imagine you need to pick between two equally attractive job offers. Making a decision may be a tiring, difficult ordeal, but once you make up your mind, something miraculous happens. Suddenly – if you are like most people – you view the chosen offer as better than you did before and conclude that the other option was not that great after all. According to social psychologist Leon Festinger, we re-evaluate the options post-choice to reduce the tension that arises from making a difficult decision between equally desirable options.

In a brain-imaging study I conducted with Ray Dolan and Benedetto De Martino in 2009, we asked subjects to imagine going on vacation to 80 different destinations and rate how happy they thought they would be in each place. We then asked them to select one destination from two choices that they had rated exactly the same. Would you choose Paris over Brazil? Finally, we asked them to imagine and rate all the destinations again. Seconds after picking between two destinations, people rated their selected destination higher than before and rated the discarded choice lower than before.

The brain-imaging data revealed that these changes were happening in the caudate nucleus, a cluster of nerve cells that is part of the striatum. The caudate has been shown to process rewards and signal their expectation. If we believe we are about to be given a paycheck or eat a scrumptious chocolate cake, the caudate acts as an announcer broadcasting to other parts of the brain, "Be ready for something good." After we receive the reward, the value is quickly updated. If there is a bonus in the paycheck, this higher value will be reflected in striatal activity. If the cake is disappointing, the decreased value will be tracked so that next time our expectations will be lower.

In our experiment, after a decision was made between two destinations, the caudate nucleus rapidly updated its signal. Before choosing, it might signal "thinking of something great" while imagining both Greece and Thailand. But after choosing Greece, it now broadcast "thinking of something remarkable!" for Greece and merely "thinking of something good" for Thailand.

True, sometimes we regret our decisions; our choices can turn out to be disappointing. But on balance, when you make a decision – even if it is a hypothetical choice – you will value it more and expect it to bring you pleasure.

This affirmation of our decisions helps us derive heightened pleasure from choices that might actually be neutral. Without this, our lives might well be filled with second-guessing. Have we done the right thing? Should we change our mind? We would find ourselves stuck, overcome by indecision and unable to move forward.

The puzzle of optimism

While the past few years have seen important advances in the neuroscience of optimism, one enduring puzzle remained. How is it that people maintain this rosy bias even when information challenging our upbeat forecasts is so readily available? Only recently have we been able to decipher this mystery, by scanning the brains of people as they process both positive and negative information about the future. The findings are striking: when people learn, their neurons faithfully encode desirable information that can enhance optimism but fail at incorporating unexpectedly undesirable information. When we hear a success story like Mark Zuckerberg's, our brains take note of the possibility that we too may become immensely rich one day. But hearing that the odds of divorce are almost one in two tends not to make us think that our own marriages may be destined to fail.

Why would our brains be wired in this way? It is tempting to speculate that optimism was selected by evolution precisely because, on balance, positive expectations enhance the odds of survival. Research findings that optimists live longer and are healthier, plus the fact that most humans display optimistic biases – and emerging data that optimism is linked to specific genes – all strongly support this hypothesis. Yet optimism is also irrational and can lead to unwanted outcomes. The question then is, How can we remain hopeful – benefiting from the fruits of optimism – while at the same time guarding ourselves from its pitfalls?

I believe knowledge is key. We are not born with an innate understanding of our biases. The brain's illusions have to be identified by careful scientific observation and controlled experiments and then communicated to the rest of us. Once we are made aware of our optimistic illusions, we can act to protect ourselves. The good news is that awareness rarely shatters the illusion. The glass remains half full. It is possible, then, to strike a balance, to believe we will stay healthy, but get medical insurance anyway; to be certain the sun will shine, but grab an umbrella on our way out — just in case.

Tali Sharot is a research fellow at University College London's Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging

© 2011 Tali Sharot


Your IP address will be logged

Comments

118 comments, displaying oldest first

or to join the conversation

  • This symbol indicates that that person is The Guardian's staffStaff
  • This symbol indicates that that person is a contributorContributor
  • ehaines

    1 January 2012 12:29AM

    I find it hard to believe that optimism can prevail considering the sorry state of this country that I live in. ;)

    Cheers Condems for a stinky 2011.

    Happy New Year.

  • irishjorge

    1 January 2012 12:30AM

    How many of these fMRI scans were on Americans, with their cultural bias towards optimism and the eternal false hope of the American dream? Dr Sharot is now based in London but her PhD is from New York university. I ownder if the same results would appear if scanning in Scotland!

  • creekwhore

    1 January 2012 12:43AM

    This comment was removed by a moderator because it didn't abide by our community standards. Replies may also be deleted. For more detail see our FAQs.

  • slicedmind

    1 January 2012 1:03AM

    The error came early on: "We are rational human beings." Oxymoron. Not an attempt at cynical wit, but the large bulk of psychology and neuroscience shows that humans are not driven by reason. It's not surprising that we err on the side of optimism rather than pessimism, by the very dint that we are still here and have loads of cool stuff around. Still, interesting to see a collation of interesting recent evidence to back up her point.

  • tysmuse

    1 January 2012 1:06AM

    The negative parts of my brain tell me that 2012 will be mainly consist of really depressing events, much like 2011.

    The optimistic side firmly believes that whatever happens... Charlie Brooker's 2012 Wipe will make me laugh at it all.

    Happy New Year everyone!

  • PseudologiaFantastic

    1 January 2012 1:09AM

    I can see already that hard-wired is going to be one of the key wank words for 2012.

    Think Dr Sharot's use of the term trivialises her own research. Hard-wired implies the opposite to what her research suggests - that is, that the brain is not readily changeable, it is hard-wired for intrinsic and relatively unmodifiable behavior patterns.

    Once pseudoscientists hear her using the term in this way, expect to spend the year fielding a flood of their arguments suggesting the brain is hard-wired for whatever their pseudoscientific agenda may be.

    Beside, this research is not new; a lot has been written on monitoring, self-regulation, the planning fallacy and so forth. Wonder if this new book is not taking advantage of the current economic climate and the increasing market for feel-good, self-help lit. Again, serious science straying into pseudoscientific territory.

  • bluejewel

    1 January 2012 1:10AM

    Once we are made aware of our optimistic illusions, we can act to protect ourselves.

    Having recent read Steven Pinker's latest book, my first thought is how to balance this against the cognitive illusion of historical myopia, i.e. that everything was so much better in the past, today is crap, and the future will be worse.

    It seems simple enough. To be able to act to protect ourselves, we need to be aware of all of our illusions.

  • antipodean1

    1 January 2012 1:16AM

    Yes - so far so good - if we weren't optimistic there wouldn't be much point getting out of bed in the morning (unless one was a supermodel of course) but the times are surely changing and humans need to move past taking an umbrella just in case to taking on the enormity of internalizing that we are now the rainmakers, with the power to overpopulate, consume the resources we depend on and and destroy the very climate itself by our (in) actions.

  • horseoutside

    1 January 2012 1:26AM

    Healthy people are optimistic, because its a broadly healthy thing to be. Its the sick psychopaths in our midst (roughly 2%) who are constantly trying to drag us all down to their sick manipulative level, and who are happily succeeding. The psychopathic mind is a dead weight on humanity.

  • Kepler

    1 January 2012 1:27AM

    There are no wires, hard or otherwise in the human body, brain or energy field. This expression is a lazy way for scientists, usually but not exclusively male, to claim what they want to.

  • Mackname

    1 January 2012 1:28AM

    I guess the secret is that we are living in an artificial world of our presumption of beings. Perhaps we are not ready yet (biologically evolved) to understand what life could be for our species.
    On one hand, we judge things according to state of our physical being and on the other hand we are coexisting collectively.

    Let me put it this way, our brain is working only as a biological simulating device susceptible to suggestion and manipulation, concern about things which might not even exist in any sense or form. The brain could mainly be a dream-machine.
    The world of finance, invention of money, religion, civilisation, etc. they are all creation of our delusional minds.

    The problem I presume is that we think we are “normal”, but I sometimes suspect that those accused as being in state of “insanity” might be more up to truth than well, us ordinary ones.

  • tomkun

    1 January 2012 1:45AM

    Judging by some of these comments some people are clearly hard-wired to be miserable sods.

  • Lewelltam

    1 January 2012 1:50AM

    Given that, without any doubt whatsoever, all of us are going to die someday, and that before that happens many of us will endure lengthy painful preludes to the end of our lives, I for one am glad that there's at least a part of my brain devoted to making me feel hopeful for the future. Were that not the case, life would be shitty in the extreme.

  • NTEightySix

    1 January 2012 2:01AM

    Optimism with a bit cautious restraint is a sound template for all of us to live up to. Undue pessimism and fatalism will only hinder our progress as human beings and is perhaps associated with certain, undesirable physical and mental health consequences.

  • Andie4

    1 January 2012 2:02AM

    Same old pseudoscience, different year..pass the prozac.
    Happy New Year Guardian readers from a Mildly Depressed Realist.

  • Lewelltam

    1 January 2012 2:06AM

    Seems that Ajit Varki, a biologist at the University of California, San Diego and I are in broad agreement.

    Yeah, I may have written that comment before reading the article.

    Sod off! I'm drunk.


    (Come back? I didn't mean to be that rude just then. Sorry. I'm drunk)

  • unhavatar

    1 January 2012 2:20AM

    Another study in the science of crapology. People are are driven by craving and greed; not hope. This the basic instinct of survival, and while most of us crave to continue in our enjoyment of sensuality for eternity, there are times when we crave not to exist and hope to die, hence the many suicides, suicidal tendencies and death wishes.

    We have hope because it goes with our craving. The author talks about such things as her ideas being hardwired, etc. Well, show me, prove it.

    It is just hot air craving to be recognized and acknowledged, the academic reality of publish or perish.

  • Erzano

    1 January 2012 2:37AM

    How many of these fMRI scans were on Americans, with their cultural bias towards optimism and the eternal false hope of the American dream? Dr Sharot is now based in London but her PhD is from New York university. I ownder if the same results would appear if scanning in Scotland!

    Did you not read the article?

    "The belief that the future will be much better than the past and present is known as the optimism bias. It abides in every race, region and socioeconomic bracket."

    After reading the responses to this article. I am now relying on my hard-wired optimistic brain not to give up hope on humanities comprehension skills. Wish me luck.

  • fripouille

    1 January 2012 2:40AM

    I sincerely don't know how I'd be able to live happily at my age (58) if I were a cynic. I did my time as a paid-up cynic when I was younger and I've done my bit. From being a young revolutionary to a workplace warrior, a starry-eyed hippy to a Porche-owning naive believer in money, you name it, I've been there, I've slagged it all off and so have many others.

    But what the hell. That's what life is. We all know that. So a bit of optimism with regard to how we may all get on better in the future is always good.

  • ranelagh75

    1 January 2012 2:44AM

    Anyone who says that human beings are hard-wired for optimism has never spent a day in this country.

  • F1geater

    1 January 2012 2:54AM

    "Even apart from the instability due to speculation, there is the instability due to the characteristic of human nature that a large proportion of our positive activities depend on spontaneous optimism rather than on a mathematical expectation, whether moral or hedonistic or economic. Most, probably, of our decisions to do something positive, the full consequences of which will be drawn out over many days to come, can only be taken as a result of animal spirits — of a spontaneous urge to action rather than inaction, and not as the outcome of a weighted average of quantitative benefits multiplied by quantitative probabilities. Enterprise only pretends to itself to be mainly actuated by the statements in its own prospectus, however candid and sincere. Only a little more than an expedition to the South Pole, is it based on an exact calculation of benefits to come. Thus if the animal spirits are dimmed and the spontaneous optimism falters, leaving us to depend on nothing but a mathematical expectation, enterprise will fade and die; — though fears of loss may have a basis no more reasonable than hopes of profit had before."

    J.M. Keynes, Chapter 12, The General Theory of Employment, Interest and Money

  • Jiminoz

    1 January 2012 3:01AM

    I can't understand all the negative coments here. The article seems quite sensible, and I assume that the neuroscience information presented will be new to at least most readers (as it is to me).

    It is also clear to me, as a pessimistically objective realist, that most people are absurdly optimistic. It is only this that keeps us going. Otherwise, we'd just polish ourselves off as soon as we were able to, to avoid the negatives of living. Also, I suggest that similar traits occur throughout the animal kingdom, though with different neural bases, as otherwise animals would just give up. This could be called a "life force" which will have it expression in different ways in different species. When this "life force" goes (as in psychotic depression) people tend to commit suicide, or just give up and die if they are already vulnerable physically. So I'd be interested to hear more about how the systems talked about are altered in depression.

    I also like the idea that the role of memory is not to accurately replay the past, but to construct possible scenarios for the future. It seems to me that one of the most important jobs for our brain is to plan how ot act in future. That is why knowing the future (which we spend a lot of time trying to do) would end up being very depressing if we knew it accurately.

  • smilerone

    1 January 2012 3:15AM

    Unfortunately I'm still hard wired like a cave dweller.

    If Talia Sharot had been my lecturer I would have carried on studying psychology as opposed to dropping it because I never really understood the difference between Freud and Jung...

  • Pigscheese

    1 January 2012 3:22AM

    "I believe in Fernando Torres."
    Oh hard luck mate as you will need more than optimism to polish that turd

    Looking on the bright side, I woke up this morning, which is a good result and far better than the alternative.
    To the eternally miserable and the naysayers I suggest you take your head out of your arses and brighten up a bit you dullards.

    Al together now 1 2 3 4
    haaaappy happy happy happy talk, talk about things we like to do
    etc etc etc

  • songlyrichere

    1 January 2012 3:24AM

    i doubt it's hard wired... doesn't that mean much more than a genetic tendency to manifest some trait? let alone a statistical tendency.

  • muggwhump

    1 January 2012 3:30AM

    This is why politicians around the world as well as the government here are so keen on the 'happiness index'.

  • songlyrichere

    1 January 2012 3:30AM

    are we hardwired to be optimistic? no. are we hard wired to tend to develop into overly optimistic people? possibly...

  • grumpypierre

    1 January 2012 3:31AM

    Jesus if that's what the rest of you are like, no wonder I'm grumpy.
    I'm going to change my username to relativelyaccuratepierre.

  • oivejoivej

    1 January 2012 3:39AM

    steady there. you have to admit though, we can tell more or less what the immediate future holds, and herein lies the difficulty of facing up to it, evidence aplenty from junk food to war, you just think it will be ok and the next thing you know you're comatose. It was much more simple before, bunga bunga, hand to mouth, circle of life, done with, there wasn't time for "psychotic depression" because "life force"dictated you were a toast. Whereas now you can spend your lifetime on life support because you're an "individual" or something. I could go on but the neighbor turned off his vile music. every day is a victory.

  • ambivabloke

    1 January 2012 3:43AM

    "The eternal false hope of the American dream", erm, the American dream trope is talked about more among the Brit chattering (and sneering) class than Americans themselves. It's never been about getting rich, it's most essential aspect has always been aspirational, and was about owning one's own home and having children who fare better than the parents..
    In the case of both "dreams", opinion polls have shown the a majority of Americans are no longer optimistic about either future being realistic. Home foreclosures and adult children moving back with their parents after college are a well recognised trends.

    So, if there's any "bias", it's Brits going on about what they think "Americans" are thinking or believing, setting aside the absurd notion that one could generalise about 340 million human beings.

  • fripouille

    1 January 2012 3:48AM

    ...you just think it will be ok and the next thing you know you're comatose. It was much more simple before, bunga bunga, hand to mouth, circle of life, done with, there wasn't time for "psychotic depression" because "life force"dictated you were a toast. Whereas now you can spend your lifetime on life support because you're an "individual" or something. I could go on but the neighbor turned off his vile music. every day is a victory.

    Hey girl, that's you, vintage. Right on the button... Oh! Music?:)

  • Andie4

    1 January 2012 3:59AM

    "The Optimism Bias"...should really be the "Denial Bias"...How are you? ...fine thanks..County in a resession ...dont worry theres a sale on at M & S....Global Warming...nah its scarmongering...Jeremy Clarkson a racist bigoted twat...nah he's just funny... De Nile...river in Egypt innit?

  • EvaninHK

    1 January 2012 4:06AM

    Hate to be negative but surely a positive outlook depends as much on external factors as it does on nature? What role does our respective cultures play? Our parents? The media and the way we are bought up to react to the media?

    Many of my relations in America seem outwardly to be happy. Outwardly they're always positive. They take everything given as a fact, and from a media that often fails to present a balanced picture. "The US transport system is the best in the world. I know it is because this is what I'm told. And I live in the US, so I am having the best that the world can offer and I should be grateful and happy." Inwardly though they fight, and often the root of their fighting is a sense of hopelessness and dis-satisfaction.

    However most of my friends across the pool are comparatively cynical. Is this because they're less optimistic or because they're more aware or manipulation? How would American or Chinese style rhetoric fall on their ears? Outwardly they hardly glow with optimism; inwardly though they seem to be happier to just take things as they come. Perhaps they're ambitions are not the same?

    And to make a further point the relations and friends I reference are in both cases ethnic Chinese, and were born and spent their formative years living in Hong Kong, though they have lived in their new homes for over decade.

    Interesting article and I will be buying the book.

  • songlyrichere

    1 January 2012 4:10AM

    i am so irrationally angry at this article.


    instead of "optimism may be hardwired by evolution into the human brain" it should surely read: "we are hard wired, to tend towards, a tendency to tend to be optimistic".

    working backwards... optimism as a trait is a tendency. not everyone has it. those that do develop it do so in dependence upon the environment.

  • Avicenna80

    1 January 2012 4:11AM

    This is alot of mumbo jumbo. A Pretty woman plugin her book on the Guardian, who would have thought of that eh. She is only interested in the publicity, as the poster above has expressed interest in buying her book.

    Life is a pile of shite, then you die. Thats all you need to know.

  • Asquith

    1 January 2012 4:20AM

    Optimists see the world as they would wish it to be. They are delusional. You'll tend to find optimists are often the same people who benefit from the status quo, hence they are smug and pleased with themselves. Cameron is optimistic, so's Blair. Guess what, they are both fabulously wealthy, hmm maybe that has something to do with their "optimism".
    Its easy to be an optimist when you're stinking rich and life smells of roses.

    There is no such thing as death, given that the only thing that exists is consciousness, ergo, suicide is simply hitting the reset button. Upon death you simply reboot in the next/other universe when your consciousness next appears (and in the infiniteness of the universe, it will). People who say "you only get one life" are talking out of their arses, do you really think you're that f****ng special, do you think the universe cannot remake you as it sees fit?

    Humans, stop thinking you're unique and special, because you ain't.

    Why is suicide seen as a bad thing? If someone is unhappy, they have every right to end their misery. It is not the suicider who is selfish, rather the selfish ones are those who insist the depressed must carry on purely so the smug happy people can feel good about themselves.

    For many human beings on this planet, there is no real optimism, unless you totally delude yourself into insanity.

    TBH I don't see why I should have to continue with the shit-river that is my life just because a bunch of ninnying selfish family members and relatives might be a bit upset (for purely selfish reasons).
    Suicide is not a tragedy, it is the end of suffering, and the beginning of a future consciousness that may find happiness. If you're uneployed, poor, or suffer social anxiety, the reset button of death is much better than life. Sometimes pessimism is the realisation of the truth, but that's something thats beyond the grasp of smug, preening middle class knob-heads.

  • songlyrichere

    1 January 2012 4:30AM

    don't kill urself ^^


    maybe we're hardwired to learn a language, and necessarily have a private one. but we're not hardwired to be optimistic. nahnahnahnah.

  • PJMolloy

    1 January 2012 4:39AM

    An interesting article, but one that gives more credit to neuroscience than it deserves, possibly. Various complex human emotions and responses are "explained" in terms of activity in specific regions of the brain - for example:

    "The findings were particularly fascinating because these precise regions – the amygdala and the rACC – show abnormal activity in depressed individuals. " "Seconds after picking between two destinations, people rated their selected destination higher than before and rated the discarded choice lower than before. The brain-imaging data revealed that these changes were happening in the caudate nucleus, a cluster of nerve cells that is part of the striatum."

    implying a direct cause-and-effect relationship, which is at best a gross oversimplification of the truth, Does activation of the amygdala cause depression, or does depression cause the amygdala to be activated? Is this relationship causal or merely synchronous? Or is there, perhaps, an intermediate process - or even a number of intermediate processes - going on somewhere else that cannot be detected or measured using MRI scanners? Just because we can only measure one kind of activity, it does not mean that other types of activity are not also taking place.

    The idea that certain areas of the brain were control centres for specific personality and behavioral traits was, after all, the basis of the now discredited "science" of phrenology, which was hailed as a great discovery in the 19th century and which had an enormous influence on the development of modern neuroscience. Indeed, the basic assumption in phrenology - that mental processes are localized in the brain - has been retained. The problem is not that there is no relationship between brain activity and behavior - clearly there is - but that there is still a tendency to describe these relationships in simplistic, cause-and-effect terms, and to interpret data - even anecdotal data, such as the alleged response of the author of this article's friend on being stuck at the airport - as supportive of whatever causal theory is being advanced. In the airport example, for instance, how can the author be sure that her friend was telling the truth when he mentions the upside of the snowstorm? He may have been merely making polite conversation when he made that remark. It might have been an ironic remark, and he actually thought the opposite. He might even have made the comment because he knew she was interested in "optimism in the face of adversity". There are many possible interpretations, yet the author confidently states: "...he quickly concludes that snow today means better skiing conditions tomorrow. His brain works to match the unexpected misfortune of being stuck at the airport to its eager anticipation of a fun getaway."
    Is that really what he "quickly concluded"? Or is it, perhaps, what the author herself concluded because it supported her thesis?
    Neuroscientists, I've noticed,have a tendency to be unrealistically optimistic in their ability to explain human behavior in terms of electrical and chemical processes in the brain.

or to join the conversation

Guardian Bookshop

This week's bestsellers

  1. 1.  Quantum Universe

    £20.00

  2. 2.  Why Does E=mc2?

    by Brian Cox £8.99

  3. 3.  God Delusion

    by Richard Dawkins £8.99

  4. 4.  Short History of Nearly Everything

    by Bill Bryson £9.99

  5. 5.  Grand Design

    by Stephen Hawking £8.99

Bestsellers from the Guardian shop