Nature | News

Scientists may be reaching a peak in reading habits

Scholarly articles in digital forms overtook printed ones, but survey suggests increase in reading may have levelled off.

Corrected:

Article tools

Rights & Permissions

Shutterstock

A survey of the reading habits of US university researchers saw a drop in the traditional, print-based consumption of information.

A 35-year trend of researchers reading ever more scholarly papers seems to be levelling off. In 2012, US scientists and social scientists estimated that they read, on average, 22 scholarly articles per month (or 264 per year). That is, statistically, not different from what they reported in an identical survey last conducted in 2005. It is the first time since the reading-habit questionnaire began in 1977 that manuscript consumption has not increased.

“People have probably hit the limit of the time they have available to read articles,” says information scientist Carol Tenopir, who led the study.

Tenopir, who heads the Center for Information and Communication Studies at the University of Tennessee in Knoxville, speculates that a wealth of other information sources is cutting away from the time scholars have to read articles in detail. The survey defines ‘reading’ as going beyond titles or abstracts to the main body of an article, and so it does not reveal whether researchers are quickly skimming over more articles than they did before.

Tenopir’s colleague Donald King began mailing out a reading-habits questionnaire to scholars in 1977. It asked them to recall details of their last scholarly reading, and to estimate the number of scholarly articles they had read in the past month. Researchers said that they got through 12–13 articles per month, and spent an average of 48 minutes on each article. Through the 1980s and 90s, they progressively reported reading more and more articles, but spending less and less time on each.

In 2005, Tenopir and King’s questionnaire found a marked leap in article numbers — which Tenopir thinks may have resulted from a huge increase in access to digitized back-files — and a decrease in the time taken per article, falling to 32 minutes. Aside from the levelling out of article readings, the latest survey of 800 scholars, which is due to appear in the journal Learned Publishing1, also finds that the time taken per article seems to have bottomed out at just over half an hour.

Going digital

Further details from the study reveal that scholars now read more than half their articles on an electronic screen, whereas in 2005, just one-fifth of readings took place on screen. However, researchers over 60 years old reported that 58% of their readings were still on paper (including printed-out versions of downloaded articles). Print readings were generally read with more care, according to the survey

The study is valuable because it stretches back such a long time, but the questions it asks may be increasingly outdated, notes David Nicholas, former head of information science at University College London, who now runs a consultancy firm, CIBER Research, in Newbury, UK. When articles were only available in print, it was implicitly assumed by communication analysts that researchers always read manuscripts in their entirety, as if a ‘scholarly article’ was an object to be consumed as a whole. That may never have been true, he says: most of the time, scholars were likely scanning for particular snippets of information.

Now that researchers can look for articles online, it is clearer that they “power-browse”, says Nicholas, bouncing through a terrain of articles with four or five browser windows open at any one time. Nicholas, who has come to his conclusions by examining log files to study the digital footprints of scholars online, says that shorter articles are more likely to be read than longer ones. “We need to burn up some of the books we wrote on information seeking and how scientists get information,” he says — and that should also apply to the way researchers write their articles. “Why are we asking people to do 100,000-word PhDs? Because long is better than short? It isn’t any more.”

Tenopir acknowledges that a day may come when her survey questions no longer fit scholarly activity, and comparisons to previous years lose meaning. The questionnaire asks: “In the past month, approximately how many scholarly articles have you read?” But, notes Tenopir, with databases, blogs and other information sources becoming increasingly important, “When will the definition of ‘an article’ become so opaque that we can’t ask the question any more?”

Journal name:
Nature
DOI:
doi:10.1038/nature.2014.14658

Corrections

Corrected:

This article’s headline and details have been corrected in response to a re-analysis of the data by the authors of the study. At first, the paper had stated that scientists were reading fewer papers in 2012 than 2005. But in response to questions raised by Phil Davis, a scholarly-publishing consultant based in Ithaca, New York, the authors examined median amounts of reading — not just the mean amounts, which could have been skewed by a few respondents with very high reading levels — and the confidence intervals around those averages. The new analysis show no statistically significant difference between the two years.

References

  1. Tenopir, C., King, D. W., Christian, L. & Volentine, R. Learned Publishing (in press) (2014).

For the best commenting experience, please login or register as a user and agree to our Community Guidelines. You will be re-directed back to this page where you will see comments updating in real-time and have the ability to recommend comments to other users.

Comments for this thread are now closed.

Comments

6 comments Subscribe to comments

  1. Avatar for Michael Lerman
    Michael Lerman
    Well known Nature, Science, and Cell rejected real discoveries, which were then published in so called second rate journals. I read everything available on the web. Michael Lerman, Ph.D., M.D.
  2. Avatar for Michael Lerman
    Michael Lerman
    I read only the Abstract and some times analyze the figures and tables. Michael Lerman, Ph.D., M.D.
  3. Avatar for Richard Van Noorden
    Richard Van Noorden
    Note to readers: this article was corrected after the three comments below were published.
  4. Avatar for Javed Mir
    Javed Mir
    --articles online,-- Because of the availability of internet facility, book reading has diminished substantially because of less pressure on the eyes since screen material can be maximised.
  5. Avatar for Andrew Barbour
    Andrew Barbour
    Did I miss something? The data suggests that 2005 was some sort of anomaly: the value for 2012 is still higher than for any other years (other than 2005).
  6. Avatar for Ishtiyaque Ahmad
    Ishtiyaque Ahmad
    In the current age of marketing, most of the reading will focus on how to sell your knowledge rather than to gain knowledge. It is incraesing being popularized that you put your knowledge to commercial gain (no harm in that) which encourages short cuts and undue conclusion from prelimnary data and knowledge. Such culture is either sidelining true scientists or forcing them to adapt a culture which is not compatible with their true nature. In either case majority of true scientists are the unfortunate loser.

Top Story

Retina

Next-generation stem cells cleared for human trial

Researchers hope to treat macular degeneration of the retina with induced pluripotent stem cells, a method that has generated enormous expectations.

Science jobs from naturejobs