1
We're unable to sign you in at this time. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
We were able to sign you in, but your subscription(s) could not be found. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
There may be a problem with your account. Please contact the AMA Service Center to resolve this issue.
Contact the AMA Service Center:
Telephone: 1 (800) 262-2350 or 1 (312) 670-7827  *   Email: subscriptions@jamanetwork.com
Error Message ......
Research Letters |

Association Between Positive Age Stereotypes and Recovery From Disability in Older Persons FREE

Becca R. Levy, PhD; Martin D. Slade, MPH; Terrence E. Murphy, PhD; Thomas M. Gill, MD
[+] Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Division of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Yale School of Public Health (Dr Levy; becca.levy@yale.edu) and Department of Internal Medicine, Yale School of Medicine (Mr Slade and Drs Murphy and Gill), New Haven, Connecticut.


Letters Section Editor: Jody W. Zylke, MD, Senior Editor.

More Author Information
JAMA. 2012;308(19):1972-1973. doi:10.1001/jama.2012.14541.
Text Size: A A A
Published online

To the Editor: Little research has been conducted on factors that account for why some older persons recover from disability and others do not. We considered a new culture-based explanatory factor: age stereotypes (defined as beliefs about old people as a category).1 Positive age stereotypes may promote recovery from disability through several pathways: limiting cardiovascular response to stress,2 improving physical balance,3 enhancing self-efficacy,4 and increasing engagement in healthy behaviors.1,4 We hypothesized that older persons with positive age stereotypes would be more likely to recover from disability than those with negative age stereotypes. Recovery was based on 4 essential activities of daily living (ADLs; bathing, dressing, transferring, and walking) that are strongly associated with use of health care services and longevity.5,6

The sample was drawn from the Precipitating Events Project, a study with high participation (75.2%) and follow-up (95.4%) rates. Participants were recruited from a health plan in greater New Haven, Connecticut, interviewed monthly for up to 129 months, and completed home-based assessments every 18 months from March 1998 through December 2008.5,6 Inclusion criteria were age of 70 years or older, English-speaking, community-living, nondisabled (ie, independent in the 4 ADLs at baseline), responded to the baseline age–stereotype measure, and experienced at least 1 month of ADL disability during follow-up (117 participants remained nondisabled). The final sample consisted of 598 participants (79.3% of the Precipitating Events Project cohort). The Yale University Human Subjects Committee approved the study; all participants provided verbal informed consent.

Age stereotypes were assessed by asking, “When you think of old persons, what are the first 5 words or phrases that come to mind?”1,4 Responses (coded on a 5-item scale ranging from 1 = most negative [eg, decrepit] to 5 = most positive [eg, spry]) were averaged and dichotomized at the mean of 2.12.1,4 Interrater reliability was 94.0%. This measure has been validated and the stereotypes have been found to be stable across time.1,4 Recovery was assessed by 3 validated ADL-improvement transitions: severe disability (needing assistance in 3 or 4 of the ADLs) to no disability (independent in all 4 ADLs), severe disability to mild disability (needing assistance in 1 or 2 of the ADLs), and mild disability to no disability.6 Covariates related to disability in previous studies5,6 included age, sex, race, education, living alone, number of physician-diagnosed chronic conditions, Mini-Mental State Examination score, depressive symptoms, and physical frailty (ie, >10 seconds on a rapid-gait test). The latter 5 covariates were updated every 18 months.

A competing risk Cox model for recurrent events6 evaluated the likelihood of the positive age–stereotype group to experience each recovery transition. This model adjusted for time participants spent in each ADL-disability state (severe, mild, or none), whether they recovered or worsened, and number of times and degree to which they recovered or worsened.6 Absolute risk increase percentages were calculated for the positive age–stereotype group. An individual growth model examined the association between age stereotypes and ADL-disability states over the 129 months. All models were adjusted for all covariates. We used SAS version 9.2 (SAS Institute Inc) and considered 2-tailed P values of less than .05 as significant.

Participants in the positive and negative age–stereotype groups were similar at baseline (Table 1).

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 1. Baseline Characteristics of Study Participants by Age-Stereotype Groups

Relative to the negative age–stereotype group, the positive age–stereotype group had the following hazard ratios for recovery transitions: severe to none, 1.44 (95% CI, 1.01 to 2.06); severe to mild, 1.23 (95% CI, 1.03 to 1.46); and mild to none, 1.15 (95% CI, 1.03 to 1.29). The positive age–stereotype group showed an advantage in the associated absolute risk increase percentages (Table 2). As indicated by the individual growth model,the age-stereotype group × time interaction (β = −0.0004 [95% CI, −0.0006 to −0.0002]; P = .001) showed that the positive age–stereotype group had a significantly slower rate of ADL decline.

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2. Association of Positive Age Stereotypes With Recovery From Disability

Older persons with positive age stereotypes were 44% more likely to fully recover from severe disability than those with negative age stereotypes. The study was limited by recruitment from 1 community; however, its demography is similar to the US population, except for an underrepresentation of black participants. Further research is needed to determine whether interventions to promote positive age stereotypes could extend independent living in later life.

Author Contributions: Dr Levy had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: Levy, Slade, Gill.

Acquisition of data: Levy, Gill.

Analysis and interpretation of data: Levy, Slade, Murphy, Gill.

Drafting of the manuscript: Levy, Slade.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Levy, Slade, Murphy, Gill.

Statistical analysis: Levy, Slade, Murphy.

Obtained funding: Levy, Gill.

Study supervision: Levy, Gill.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: The authors have completed and submitted the ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest and none were reported.

Funding/Support: Research for this report was funded by National Institutes on Health grants R01AG032284 and R01HL089314 (Dr Levy) and R37AG17560, R01AG023993, and K24AG021507 (Dr Gill). In addition, Dr Levy was supported by an investigator award from the Patrick and Catherine Weldon Donaghue Medical Research Foundation. The Precipitating Events Project was conducted at the Yale Claude D. Pepper Older Americans Independence Center (grant P30AG21342).

Role of the Sponsor: The sources of funding and support had no role in the design and conduct of the study; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Levy B, Langer E. Aging free from negative stereotypes: successful memory in China and among the American Deaf.  J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;66(6):989-997
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Levy BR, Hausdorff JM, Hencke R, Wei JY. Reducing cardiovascular stress with positive self-stereotypes of aging.  J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2000;55(4):205-213
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Levy BR, Leifheit-Limson E. The stereotype-matching effect: greater influence on functioning when age stereotypes correspond to outcomes.  Psychol Aging. 2009;24(1):230-233
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Levy BR. Stereotype embodiment: a psychosocial approach to aging.  Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;18(6):332-336
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Hardy SE, Gill TM. Recovery from disability among community-dwelling older persons.  JAMA. 2004;291(13):1596-1602
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Gill TM, Allore HG, Gahbauer EA, Murphy TE. Change in disability after hospitalization or restricted activity in older persons.  JAMA. 2010;304(17):1919-1928
PubMed   |  Link to Article

Figures

Tables

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 1. Baseline Characteristics of Study Participants by Age-Stereotype Groups
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2. Association of Positive Age Stereotypes With Recovery From Disability

References

Levy B, Langer E. Aging free from negative stereotypes: successful memory in China and among the American Deaf.  J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;66(6):989-997
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Levy BR, Hausdorff JM, Hencke R, Wei JY. Reducing cardiovascular stress with positive self-stereotypes of aging.  J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2000;55(4):205-213
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Levy BR, Leifheit-Limson E. The stereotype-matching effect: greater influence on functioning when age stereotypes correspond to outcomes.  Psychol Aging. 2009;24(1):230-233
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Levy BR. Stereotype embodiment: a psychosocial approach to aging.  Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;18(6):332-336
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Hardy SE, Gill TM. Recovery from disability among community-dwelling older persons.  JAMA. 2004;291(13):1596-1602
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Gill TM, Allore HG, Gahbauer EA, Murphy TE. Change in disability after hospitalization or restricted activity in older persons.  JAMA. 2010;304(17):1919-1928
PubMed   |  Link to Article
CME
Meets CME requirements for:
Browse CME for all U.S. States
Accreditation Information
The American Medical Association is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education to provide continuing medical education for physicians. The AMA designates this journal-based CME activity for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM per course. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. Physicians who complete the CME course and score at least 80% correct on the quiz are eligible for AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM.
Note: You must get at least of the answers correct to pass this quiz.
You have not filled in all the answers to complete this quiz
The following questions were not answered:
Sorry, you have unsuccessfully completed this CME quiz with a score of
The following questions were not answered correctly:
Commitment to Change (optional):
Indicate what change(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Your quiz results:
The filled radio buttons indicate your responses. The preferred responses are highlighted
For CME Course: A Proposed Model for Initial Assessment and Management of Acute Heart Failure Syndromes
Indicate what changes(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.

Multimedia

Some tools below are only available to our subscribers or users with an online account.

Web of Science® Times Cited: 8

Related Content

Customize your page view by dragging & repositioning the boxes below.

Articles Related By Topic
Related Collections
PubMed Articles